Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • My Account Login
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Open access
  • Published: 08 June 2021

Metacognition: ideas and insights from neuro- and educational sciences

  • Damien S. Fleur   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4836-5255 1 , 2 ,
  • Bert Bredeweg   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5281-2786 1 , 3 &
  • Wouter van den Bos 2 , 4  

npj Science of Learning volume  6 , Article number:  13 ( 2021 ) Cite this article

34k Accesses

44 Citations

11 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Human behaviour
  • Interdisciplinary studies

Metacognition comprises both the ability to be aware of one’s cognitive processes (metacognitive knowledge) and to regulate them (metacognitive control). Research in educational sciences has amassed a large body of evidence on the importance of metacognition in learning and academic achievement. More recently, metacognition has been studied from experimental and cognitive neuroscience perspectives. This research has started to identify brain regions that encode metacognitive processes. However, the educational and neuroscience disciplines have largely developed separately with little exchange and communication. In this article, we review the literature on metacognition in educational and cognitive neuroscience and identify entry points for synthesis. We argue that to improve our understanding of metacognition, future research needs to (i) investigate the degree to which different protocols relate to the similar or different metacognitive constructs and processes, (ii) implement experiments to identify neural substrates necessary for metacognition based on protocols used in educational sciences, (iii) study the effects of training metacognitive knowledge in the brain, and (iv) perform developmental research in the metacognitive brain and compare it with the existing developmental literature from educational sciences regarding the domain-generality of metacognition.

Similar content being viewed by others

metacognitive knowledge essay

The language network as a natural kind within the broader landscape of the human brain

metacognitive knowledge essay

Sleep quality, duration, and consistency are associated with better academic performance in college students

metacognitive knowledge essay

Artificial intelligence and illusions of understanding in scientific research

Introduction.

Metacognition is defined as “thinking about thinking” or the ability to monitor and control one’s cognitive processes 1 and plays an important role in learning and education 2 , 3 , 4 . For instance, high performers tend to present better metacognitive abilities (especially control) than low performers in diverse educational activities 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 . Recently, there has been a lot of progress in studying the neural mechanisms of metacognition 10 , 11 , yet it is unclear at this point how these results may inform educational sciences or interventions. Given the potential benefits of metacognition, it is important to get a better understanding of how metacognition works and of how training can be useful.

The interest in bridging cognitive neuroscience and educational practices has increased in the past two decades, spanning a large number of studies grouped under the umbrella term of educational neuroscience 12 , 13 , 14 . With it, researchers have brought forward issues that are viewed as critical for the discipline to improve education. Recurring issues that may impede the relevance of neural insights for educational practices concern external validity 15 , 16 , theoretical discrepancies 17 and differences in terms of the domains of (meta)cognition operationalised (specific or general) 15 . This is important because, in recent years, brain research is starting to orient itself towards training metacognitive abilities that would translate into real-life benefits. However, direct links between metacognition in the brain and metacognition in domains such as education have still to be made. As for educational sciences, a large body of literature on metacognitive training is available, yet we still need clear insights about what works and why. While studies suggest that training metacognitive abilities results in higher academic achievement 18 , other interventions show mixed results 19 , 20 . Moreover, little is known about the long-term effects of, or transfer effects, of these interventions. A better understanding of the cognitive processes involved in metacognition and how they are expressed in the brain may provide insights in these regards.

Within cognitive neuroscience, there has been a long tradition of studying executive functions (EF), which are closely related to metacognitive processes 21 . Similar to metacognition, EF shows a positive relationship with learning at school. For instance, performance in laboratory tasks involving error monitoring, inhibition and working memory (i.e. processes that monitor and regulate cognition) are associated with academic achievement in pre-school children 22 . More recently, researchers have studied metacognition in terms of introspective judgements about performance in a task 10 . Although the neural correlates of such behaviour are being revealed 10 , 11 , little is known about how behaviour during such tasks relates to academic achievement.

Educational and cognitive neuroscientists study metacognition in different contexts using different methods. Indeed, while the latter investigate metacognition via behavioural task, the former mainly rely on introspective questionnaires. The extent to which these different operationalisations of metacognition match and reflect the same processes is unclear. As a result, the external validity of methodologies used in cognitive neuroscience is also unclear 16 . We argue that neurocognitive research on metacognition has a lot of potential to provide insights in mechanisms relevant in educational contexts, and that theoretical and methodological exchange between the two disciplines can benefit neuroscientific research in terms of ecological validity.

For these reasons, we investigate the literature through the lenses of external validity, theoretical discrepancies, domain generality and metacognitive training. Research on metacognition in cognitive neuroscience and educational sciences are reviewed separately. First, we investigate how metacognition is operationalised with respect to the common framework introduced by Nelson and Narens 23 (see Fig. 1 ). We then discuss the existing body of evidence regarding metacognitive training. Finally, we compare findings in both fields, highlight gaps and shortcomings, and propose avenues for research relying on crossovers of the two disciplines.

figure 1

Meta-knowledge is characterised as the upward flow from object-level to meta-level. Meta-control is characterised as the downward flow from meta-level to object-level. Metacognition is therefore conceptualised as the bottom-up monitoring and top-down control of object-level processes. Adapted from Nelson and Narens’ cognitive psychology model of metacognition 23 .

In cognitive neuroscience, metacognition is divided into two main components 5 , 24 , which originate from the seminal works of Flavell on metamemory 25 , 26 . First, metacognitive knowledge (henceforth, meta-knowledge) is defined as the knowledge individuals have of their own cognitive processes and their ability to monitor and reflect on them. Second, metacognitive control (henceforth, meta-control) consists of someone’s self-regulatory mechanisms, such as planning and adapting behaviour based on outcomes 5 , 27 . Following Nelson and Narens’ definition 23 , meta-knowledge is characterised as the flow and processing of information from the object level to the meta-level, and meta-control as the flow from the meta-level to the object level 28 , 29 , 30 (Fig. 1 ). The object-level encompasses cognitive functions such as recognition and discrimination of objects, decision-making, semantic encoding, and spatial representation. On the meta-level, information originating from the object level is processed and top-down regulation on object-level functions is imposed 28 , 29 , 30 .

Educational researchers have mainly investigated metacognition through the lens of Self-Regulated Learning theory (SRL) 3 , 4 , which shares common conceptual roots with the theoretical framework used in cognitive neuroscience but varies from it in several ways 31 . First, SRL is constrained to learning activities, usually within educational settings. Second, metacognition is merely one of three components, with “motivation to learn” and “behavioural processes”, that enable individuals to learn in a self-directed manner 3 . In SRL, metacognition is defined as setting goals, planning, organising, self-monitoring and self-evaluating “at various points during the acquisition” 3 . The distinction between meta-knowledge and meta-control is not formally laid down although reference is often made to a “self-oriented feedback loop” describing the relationship between reflecting and regulating processes that resembles Nelson and Narens’ model (Fig. 1 ) 3 , 23 . In order to facilitate the comparison of operational definitions, we will refer to meta-knowledge in educational sciences when protocols operationalise self-awareness and knowledge of strategies, and to meta-control when they operationalise the selection and use of learning strategies and planning. For an in-depth discussion on metacognition and SRL, we refer to Dinsmore et al. 31 .

Metacognition in cognitive neuroscience

Operational definitions.

In cognitive neuroscience, research in metacognition is split into two tracks 32 . One track mainly studies meta-knowledge by investigating the neural basis of introspective judgements about one’s own cognition (i.e., metacognitive judgements), and meta-control with experiments involving cognitive offloading. In these experiments, subjects can perform actions such as set reminders, making notes and delegating tasks 33 , 34 , or report their desire for them 35 . Some research has investigated how metacognitive judgements can influence subsequent cognitive behaviour (i.e., a downward stream from the meta-level to the object level), but only one study so far has explored how this relationship is mapped in the brain 35 . In the other track, researchers investigate EF, also referred to as cognitive control 30 , 36 , which is closely related to metacognition. Note however that EF are often not framed in metacognitive terms in the literature 37 (but see ref. 30 ). For the sake of concision, we limit our review to operational definitions that have been used in neuroscientific studies.

Metacognitive judgements

Cognitive neuroscientists have been using paradigms in which subjects make judgements on how confident they are with regards to their learning of some given material 10 . These judgements are commonly referred to as metacognitive judgements , which can be viewed as a form of meta-knowledge (for reviews see Schwartz 38 and Nelson 39 ). Historically, researchers mostly resorted to paradigms known as Feelings of Knowing (FOK) 40 and Judgements of Learning (JOL) 41 . FOK reflect the belief of a subject to knowing the answer to a question or a problem and being able to recognise it from a list of alternatives, despite being unable to explicitly recall it 40 . Here, metacognitive judgement is thus made after retrieval attempt. In contrast, JOL are prospective judgements during learning of one’s ability to successfully recall an item on subsequent testing 41 .

More recently, cognitive neuroscientists have used paradigms in which subjects make retrospective metacognitive judgements on their performance in a two-alternative Forced Choice task (2-AFC) 42 . In 2-AFCs, subjects are asked to choose which of two presented options has the highest criterion value. Different domains can be involved, such as perception (e.g., visual or auditory) and memory. For example, subjects may be instructed to visually discriminate which one of two boxes contains more dots 43 , identify higher contrast Gabor patches 44 , or recognise novel words from words that were previously learned 45 (Fig. 2 ). The subjects engage in metacognitive judgements by rating how confident they are relative to their decision in the task. Based on their responses, one can evaluate a subject’s metacognitive sensitivity (the ability to discriminate one’s own correct and incorrect judgements), metacognitive bias (the overall level of confidence during a task), and metacognitive efficiency (the level of metacognitive sensitivity when controlling for task performance 46 ; Fig. 3 ). Note that sensitivity and bias are independent aspects of metacognition, meaning that two subjects may display the same levels of metacognitive sensitivity, but one may be biased towards high confidence while the other is biased towards low confidence. Because metacognitive sensitivity is affected by the difficulty of the task (one subject tends to display greater metacognitive sensitivity in easy tasks than difficult ones and different subjects may find a task more or less easy), metacognitive efficiency is an important measure as it allows researchers to compare metacognitive abilities between subjects and between domains. The most commonly used methods to assess metacognitive sensitivity during retrospective judgements are the receiver operating curve (ROC) and meta- d ′. 46 Both derive from signal detection theory (SDT) 47 which allows Type 1 sensitivity, or d’ ′ (how a subject can discriminate between stimulus alternatives, i.e. object-level processes) to be differentiated from metacognitive sensitivity (a judgement on the correctness of this decision) 48 . Importantly, only comparing meta- d ′ to d ′ seems to give reliable assessments metacognitive efficiency 49 . A ratio of 1 between meta- d’ ′ and d’ ′, indicates that a subject was perfectly able to discriminate between their correct and incorrect judgements. A ratio of 0.8 suggests that 80% of the task-related sensory evidence was available for the metacognitive judgements. Table 1 provides an overview of the different types of tasks and protocols with regards to the type of metacognitive process they operationalise. These operationalisations of meta-knowledge are used in combination with brain imaging methods (functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging; fMRI; MRI) to identify brain regions associated with metacognitive activity and metacognitive abilities 10 , 50 . Alternatively, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can be used to temporarily deactivate chosen brain regions and test whether this affects metacognitive abilities in given tasks 51 , 52 .

figure 2

a Visual perception task: subjects choose the box containing the most (randomly generated) dots. Subjects then rate their confidence in their decision. b Memory task: subjects learn a list of words. In the next screen, they have to identify which of two words shown was present on the list. The subjects then rate their confidence in their decision.

figure 3

The red and blue curves represent the distribution of confidence ratings for incorrect and correct trials, respectively. A larger distance between the two curves denotes higher sensitivity. Displacement to the left and right denote biases towards low confidence (low metacognitive bias) and high confidence (high metacognitive bias), respectively (retrieved from Fig. 1 in Fleming and Lau 46 ). We repeat the disclaimer of the original authors that this figure is not a statistically accurate description of correct and incorrect responses, which are typically not normally distributed 46 , 47 .

A recent meta-analysis analysed 47 neuroimaging studies on metacognition and identified a domain-general network associated with high vs. low confidence ratings in both decision-making tasks (perception 2-AFC) and memory tasks (JOL, FOK) 11 . This network includes the medial and lateral prefrontal cortex (mPFC and lPFC, respectively), precuneus and insula. In contrast, the right anterior dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) was specifically involved in decision-making tasks, and the bilateral parahippocampal cortex was specific to memory tasks. In addition, prospective judgements were associated with the posterior mPFC, left dlPFC and right insula, whereas retrospective judgements were associated with bilateral parahippocampal cortex and left inferior frontal gyrus. Finally, emerging evidence suggests a role of the right rostrolateral PFC (rlPFC) 53 , 54 , anterior PFC (aPFC) 44 , 45 , 55 , 56 , dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) 54 , 55 and precuneus 45 , 55 in metacognitive sensitivity (meta- d ′, ROC). In addition, several studies suggest that the aPFC relates to metacognition specifically in perception-related 2-AFC tasks, whereas the precuneus is engaged specifically in memory-related 2-AFC tasks 45 , 55 , 56 . This may suggest that metacognitive processes engage some regions in a domain-specific manner, while other regions are domain-general. For educational scientists, this could mean that some domains of metacognition may be more relevant for learning and, granted sufficient plasticity of the associated brain regions, that targeting them during interventions may show more substantial benefits. Note that rating one’s confidence and metacognitive sensitivity likely involve additional, peripheral cognitive processes instead of purely metacognitive ones. These regions are therefore associated with metacognition but not uniquely per se. Notably, a recent meta-analysis 50 suggests that domain-specific and domain-general signals may rather share common circuitry, but that their neural signature varies depending on the type of task or activity, showing that domain-generality in metacognition is complex and still needs to be better understood.

In terms of the role of metacognitive judgements on future behaviour, one study found that brain patterns associated with the desire for cognitive offloading (i.e., meta-control) partially overlap with those associated with meta-knowledge (metacognitive judgements of confidence), suggesting that meta-control is driven by either non-metacognitive, in addition to metacognitive, processes or by a combination of different domain-specific meta-knowledge processes 35 .

Executive function

In EF, processes such as error detection/monitoring and effort monitoring can be related to meta-knowledge while error correction, inhibitory control, and resource allocation can be related to meta-control 36 . To activate these processes, participants are asked to perform tasks in laboratory settings such as Flanker tasks, Stroop tasks, Demand Selection tasks and Motion Discrimination tasks (Fig. 4 ). Neural correlates of EF are investigated by having subjects perform such tasks while their brain activity is recorded with fMRI or electroencephalography (EEG). Additionally, patients with brain lesions can be tested against healthy participants to evaluate the functional role of the impaired regions 57 .

figure 4

a Flanker task: subjects indicate the direction to which the arrow in the middle points. b Stroop task: subjects are presented with the name of colour printed in a colour that either matches or mismatches the name. Subjects are asked to give the name of the written colour or the printed colour. c Motion Discrimination task: subjects have to determine in which direction the dots are going with variating levels of noise. d Example of a Demand Selection task: in both options subjects have to switch between two tasks. Task one, subjects determine whether the number shown is higher or lower than 5. Task two, subjects determine whether the number is odd or even. The two options (low and high demand) differ in their degree of task switching, meaning the effort required. Subjects are allowed to switch between the two options. Note, the type of task is solely indicated by the colour of the number and that the subjects are not explicitly told about the difference in effort between the two options (retrieved from Fig. 1c in Froböse et al. 58 ).

In a review article on the neural basis of EF (in which they are defined as meta-control), Shimamura argues that a network of regions composed of the aPFC, ACC, ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC) and dlPFC is involved in the regulations of cognition 30 . These regions are not only interconnected but are also intricately connected to cortical and subcortical regions outside of the PFC. The vlPFC was shown to play an important role in “selecting and maintaining information in working memory”, whereas the dlPFC is involved in “manipulating and updating information in working memory” 30 . The ACC has been proposed to monitor cognitive conflict (e.g. in a Stroop task or a Flanker task), and the dlPFC to regulate it 58 , 59 . In particular, activity in the ACC in conflict monitoring (meta-knowledge) seems to contribute to control of cognition (meta-control) in the dlPFC 60 , 61 and to “bias behavioural decision-making toward cognitively efficient tasks and strategies” (p. 356) 62 . In a recent fMRI study, subjects performed a motion discrimination task (Fig. 4c ) 63 . After deciding on the direction of the motion, they were presented additional motion (i.e. post-decisional evidence) and then were asked to rate their confidence in their initial choice. The post-decisional evidence was encoded in the activity of the posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC; meta-knowledge), while lateral aPFC (meta-control) modulated the impact of this evidence on subsequent confidence rating 63 . Finally, results from a meta-analysis study on cognitive control identified functional connectivity between the pMFC, associated with monitoring and informing other regions about the need for regulation, and the lPFC that would effectively regulate cognition 64 .

Online vs. offline metacognition

While the processes engaged during tasks such as those used in EF research can be considered as metacognitive in the sense that they are higher-order functions that monitor and control lower cognitive processes, scientists have argued that they are not functionally equivalent to metacognitive judgements 10 , 11 , 65 , 66 . Indeed, engaging in metacognitive judgements requires subjects to reflect on past or future activities. As such, metacognitive judgements can be considered as offline metacognitive processes. In contrast, high-order processes involved in decision-making tasks such as used in EF research are arguably largely made on the fly, or online , at a rapid pace and subjects do not need to reflect on their actions to perform them. Hence, we propose to explicitly distinguish online and offline processes. Other researchers have shared a similar view and some have proposed models for metacognition that make similar distinctions 65 , 66 , 67 , 68 . The functional difference between online and offline metacognition is supported by some evidence. For instance, event-related brain potential (ERP) studies suggest that error negativities are associated with error detection in general, whereas an increased error positivity specifically encodes error that subjects could report upon 69 , 70 . Furthermore, brain-imaging studies suggest that the MFC and ACC are involved in online meta-knowledge, while the aPFC and lPFC seem to be activated when subjects engage in more offline meta-knowledge and meta-control, respectively 63 , 71 , 72 . An overview of the different tasks can be found in Table 1 and a list of different studies on metacognition can be found in Supplementary Table 1 (organised in terms of the type of processes investigated, the protocols and brain measures used, along with the brain regions identified). Figure 5 illustrates the different brain regions associated with meta-knowledge and meta-control, distinguishing between what we consider to be online and offline processes. This distinction is often not made explicitly but it will be specifically helpful when building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and educational sciences.

figure 5

The regions are divided into online meta-knowledge and meta-control, and offline meta-knowledge and meta-control following the distinctions introduced earlier. Some regions have been reported to be related to both offline and online processes and are therefore given a striped pattern.

Training metacognition

There are extensive accounts in the literature of efforts to improve EF components such as inhibitory control, attention shifting and working memory 22 . While working memory does not directly reflect metacognitive abilities, its training is often hypothesised to improve general cognitive abilities and academic achievement. However, most meta-analyses found that training methods lead only to weak, non-lasting effects on cognitive control 73 , 74 , 75 . One meta-analysis did find evidence of near-transfer following EF training in children (in particular working memory, inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility), but found no evidence of far-transfer 20 . According to this study, training on one component leads to improved abilities in that same component but not in other EF components. Regarding adults, however, one meta-analysis suggests that EF training in general and working memory training specifically may both lead to significant near- and far-transfer effects 76 . On a neural level, a meta-analysis showed that cognitive training resulted in decreased brain activity in brain regions associated with EF 77 . According to the authors, this indicates that “training interventions reduce demands on externally focused attention” (p. 193) 77 .

With regards to meta-knowledge, several studies have reported increased task-related metacognitive abilities after training. For example, researchers found that subjects who received feedback on their metacognitive judgements regarding a perceptual decision-making task displayed better metacognitive accuracy, not only in the trained task but also in an untrained memory task 78 . Related, Baird and colleagues 79 found that a two-week mindfulness meditation training lead to enhanced meta-knowledge in the memory domain, but not the perceptual domain. The authors link these results to evidence of increased grey matter density in the aPFC in meditation practitioners.

Research on metacognition in cognitive science has mainly been studied through the lens of metacognitive judgements and EF (specifically performance monitoring and cognitive control). Meta-knowledge is commonly activated in subjects by asking them to rate their confidence in having successfully performed a task. A distinction is made between metacognitive sensitivity, metacognitive bias and metacognitive efficacy. Monitoring and regulating processes in EF are mainly operationalised with behavioural tasks such as Flanker tasks, Stroop tasks, Motion Discrimination tasks and Demand Selection tasks. In addition, metacognitive judgements can be viewed as offline processes in that they require the subject to reflect on her cognition and develop meta-representations. In contrast, EF can be considered as mostly online metacognitive processes because monitoring and regulation mostly happen rapidly without the need for reflective thinking.

Although there is some evidence for domain specificity, other studies have suggested that there is a single network of regions involved in all meta-cognitive tasks, but differentially activated in different task contexts. Comparing research on meta-knowledge and meta-control also suggest that some regions play a crucial role in both knowledge and regulation (Fig. 5 ). We have also identified a specific set of regions that are involved in either offline or online meta-knowledge. The evidence in favour of metacognitive training, while mixed, is interesting. In particular, research on offline meta-knowledge training involving self-reflection and metacognitive accuracy has shown some promising results. The regions that show structural changes after training, were those that we earlier identified as being part of the metacognition network. EF training does seem to show far-transfer effects at least in adults, but the relevance for everyday life activity is still unclear.

One major limitation of current research in metacognition is ecological validity. It is unclear to what extent the operationalisations reviewed above reflect real-life metacognition. For instance, are people who can accurately judge their performance on a behavioural task also able to accurately assess how they performed during an exam? Are people with high levels of error regulation and inhibitory control able to learn more efficiently? Note that criticism on the ecological validity of neurocognitive operationalisations extends beyond metacognition research 16 . A solution for improving validity may be to compare operationalisations of metacognition in cognitive neuroscience with the ones in educational sciences, which have shown clear links with learning in formal education. This also applies to metacognitive training.

Metacognition in educational sciences

The most popular protocols used to measure metacognition in educational sciences are self-report questionnaires or interviews, learning journals and thinking-aloud protocols 31 , 80 . During interviews, subjects are asked to answer questions regarding hypothetical situations 81 . In learning journals, students write about their learning experience and their thoughts on learning 82 , 83 . In thinking-aloud protocols, subjects are asked to verbalise their thoughts while performing a problem-solving task 80 . Each of these instruments can be used to study meta-knowledge and meta-control. For instance, one of the most widely used questionnaires, the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) 42 , operationalises “Flavellian” metacognition and has dedicated scales for meta-knowledge and meta-control (also popular are the MSLQ 84 and LASSI 85 which operate under SRL). The meta-knowledge scale of the MAI operationalises knowledge of strategies (e.g., “ I am aware of what strategies I use when I study ”) and self-awareness (e.g., “ I am a good judge of how well I understand something ”); the meta-control scale operationalises planning (e.g., “ I set a goal before I begin a task ”) and use of learning strategies (e.g., “ I summarize what I’ve learned after I finish ”). Learning journals, self-report questionnaires and interviews involve offline metacognition. Thinking aloud, though not engaging the same degree self-reflection, also involves offline metacognition in the sense that online processes are verbalised, which necessitate offline processing (see Table 1 for an overview and Supplementary Table 2 for more details).

More recently, methodologies borrowed from cognitive neuroscience have been introduced to study EF in educational settings 22 , 86 . In particular, researchers used classic cognitive control tasks such as the Stroop task (for a meta-analysis 86 ). Most of the studied components are related to meta-control and not meta-knowledge. For instance, the BRIEF 87 is a questionnaire completed by parents and teachers which assesses different subdomains of EF: (1) inhibition, shifting, and emotional control which can be viewed as online metacognitive control, and (2) planning, organisation of materials, and monitoring, which can be viewed as offline meta-control 87 .

Assessment of metacognition is usually compared against metrics of academic performance such as grades or scores on designated tasks. A recent meta-analysis reported a weak correlation of self-report questionnaires and interviews with academic performance whereas think-aloud protocols correlated highly 88 . Offline meta-knowledge processes operationalised by learning journals were found to be positively associated with academic achievement when related to reflection on learning activities but negatively associated when related to reflection on learning materials, indicating that the type of reflection is important 89 . EF have been associated with abilities in mathematics (mainly) and reading comprehension 86 . However, the literature points towards contrary directions as to what specific EF component is involved in academic achievement. This may be due to the different groups that were studied, to different operationalisations or to different theoretical underpinnings for EF 86 . For instance, online and offline metacognitive processes, which are not systematically distinguished in the literature, may play different roles in academic achievement. Moreover, the bulk of research focussed on young children with few studies on adolescents 86 and EF may play a role at varying extents at different stages of life.

A critical question in educational sciences is that of the nature of the relationship between metacognition and academic achievement to understand whether learning at school can be enhanced by training metacognitive abilities. Does higher metacognition lead to higher academic achievement? Do these features evolve in parallel? Developmental research provides valuable insights into the formation of metacognitive abilities that can inform training designs in terms of what aspect of metacognition should be supported and the age at which interventions may yield the best results. First, meta-knowledge seems to emerge around the age of 5, meta-control around 8, and both develop over the years 90 , with evidence for the development of meta-knowledge into adolescence 91 . Furthermore, current theories propose that meta-knowledge abilities are initially highly domain-dependent and gradually become more domain-independent as knowledge and experience are acquired and linked between domains 32 . Meta-control is believed to evolve in a similar fashion 90 , 92 .

Common methods used to train offline metacognition are direct instruction of metacognition, metacognitive prompts and learning journals. In addition, research has been done on the use of (self-directed) feedback as a means to induce self-reflection in students, mainly in computer-supported settings 93 . Interestingly, learning journals appear to be used for both assessing and fostering metacognition. Metacognitive instruction consists of teaching learners’ strategies to “activate” their metacognition. Metacognitive prompts most often consist of text pieces that are sent at specific times and that trigger reflection (offline meta-knowledge) on learning behaviour in the form of a question, hint or reminder.

Meta-analyses have investigated the effects of direct metacognitive instruction on students’ use of learning strategies and academic outcomes 18 , 94 , 95 . Their findings show that metacognitive instruction can have a positive effect on learning abilities and achievement within a population ranging from primary schoolers to university students. In particular, interventions lead to the highest effect sizes when they both (i) instructed a combination of metacognitive strategies with an emphasis on planning strategies (offline meta-control) and (ii) “provided students with knowledge about strategies” (offline meta-knowledge) and “illustrated the benefits of applying the trained strategies, or even stimulated metacognitive reasoning” (p.114) 18 . The longer the duration of the intervention, the more effective they were. The strongest effects on academic performance were observed in the context of mathematics, followed by reading and writing.

While metacognitive prompts and learning journals make up the larger part of the literature on metacognitive training 96 , meta-analyses that specifically investigate their effectiveness have yet to be performed. Nonetheless, evidence suggests that such interventions can be successful. Researchers found that metacognitive prompts fostered the use of metacognitive strategies (offline meta-control) and that the combination of cognitive and metacognitive prompts improved learning outcomes 97 . Another experiment showed that students who received metacognitive prompts performed more metacognitive activities inside the learning environment and displayed better transfer performance immediately after the intervention 98 . A similar study using self-directed prompts showed enhanced transfer performance that was still observable 3 weeks after the intervention 99 .

Several studies suggest that learning journals can positively enhance metacognition. Subjects who kept a learning journal displayed stronger high meta-control and meta-knowledge on learning tasks and tended to reach higher academic outcomes 100 , 101 , 102 . However, how the learning journal is used seems to be critical; good instructions are crucial 97 , 103 , and subjects who simply summarise their learning activity benefit less from the intervention than subjects who reflect about their knowledge, learning and learning goals 104 . An overview of studies using learning journals and metacognitive prompts to train metacognition can be found in Supplementary Table 3 .

In recent years, educational neuroscience researchers have tried to determine whether training and improvements in EF can lead to learning facilitation and higher academic achievement. Training may consist of having students continually perform behavioural tasks either in the lab, at home, or at school. Current evidence in favour of training EF is mixed, with only anecdotal evidence for positive effects 105 . A meta-analysis did not show evidence for a causal relationship between EF and academic achievement 19 , but suggested that the relationship is bidirectional, meaning that the two are “mutually supportive” 106 .

A recent review article has identified several gaps and shortcoming in the literature on metacognitive training 96 . Overall, research in metacognitive training has been mainly invested in developing learners’ meta-control rather than meta-knowledge. Furthermore, most of the interventions were done in the context of science learning. Critically, there appears to be a lack of studies that employed randomised control designs, such that the effects of metacognitive training intervention are often difficult to evaluate. In addition, research overwhelmingly investigated metacognitive prompts and learning journals in adults 96 , while interventions on EF mainly focused on young children 22 . Lastly, meta-analyses evaluating the effectiveness of metacognitive training have so far focused on metacognitive instruction on children. There is thus a clear disbalance between the meta-analyses performed and the scope of the literature available.

An important caveat of educational sciences research is that metacognition is not typically framed in terms of online and offline metacognition. Therefore, it can be unclear whether protocols operationalise online or offline processes and whether interventions tend to benefit more online or offline metacognition. There is also confusion in terms of what processes qualify as EF and definitions of it vary substantially 86 . For instance, Clements and colleagues mention work on SRL to illustrate research in EF in relation to academic achievement but the two spawn from different lines of research, one rooted in metacognition and socio-cognitive theory 31 and the other in the cognitive (neuro)science of decision-making. In addition, the MSLQ, as discussed above, assesses offline metacognition along with other components relevant to SRL, whereas EF can be mainly understood as online metacognition (see Table 1 ), which on the neural level may rely on different circuitry.

Investigating offline metacognition tends to be carried out in school settings whereas evaluating EF (e.g., Stroop task, and BRIEF) is performed in the lab. Common to all protocols for offline metacognition is that they consist of a form of self-report from the learner, either during the learning activity (thinking-aloud protocols) or after the learning activity (questionnaires, interviews and learning journals). Questionnaires are popular protocols due to how easy they are to administer but have been criticised to provide biased evaluations of metacognitive abilities. In contrast, learning journals evaluate the degree to which learners engage in reflective thinking and may therefore be less prone to bias. Lastly, it is unclear to what extent thinking-aloud protocols are sensitive to online metacognitive processes, such as on-the-fly error correction and effort regulation. The strength of the relationship between metacognitive abilities and academic achievement varies depending on how metacognition is operationalised. Self-report questionnaires and interviews are weakly related to achievement whereas thinking-aloud protocols and EF are strongly related to it.

Based on the well-documented relationship between metacognition and academic achievement, educational scientists hypothesised that fostering metacognition may improve learning and academic achievement, and thus performed metacognitive training interventions. The most prevalent training protocols are direct metacognitive instruction, learning journals, and metacognitive prompts, which aim to induce and foster offline metacognitive processes such as self-reflection, planning and selecting learning strategies. In addition, researchers have investigated whether training EF, either through tasks or embedded in the curriculum, results in higher academic proficiency and achievement. While a large body of evidence suggests that metacognitive instruction, learning journals and metacognitive prompts can successfully improve academic achievement, interventions designed around EF training show mixed results. Future research investigating EF training in different age categories may clarify this situation. These various degrees of success of interventions may indicate that offline metacognition is more easily trainable than online metacognition and plays a more important role in educational settings. Investigating the effects of different methods, offline and online, on the neural level, may provide researchers with insights into the trainability of different metacognitive processes.

In this article, we reviewed the literature on metacognition in educational sciences and cognitive neuroscience with the aim to investigate gaps in current research and propose ways to address them through the exchange of insights between the two disciplines and interdisciplinary approaches. The main aspects analysed were operational definitions of metacognition and metacognitive training, through the lens of metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive control. Our review also highlighted an additional construct in the form of the distinction between online metacognition (on the fly and largely automatic) and offline metacognition (slower, reflective and requiring meta-representations). In cognitive neuroscience, research has focused on metacognitive judgements (mainly offline) and EF (mainly online). Metacognition is operationalised with tasks carried out in the lab and are mapped onto brain functions. In contrast, research in educational sciences typically measures metacognition in the context of learning activities, mostly in schools and universities. More recently, EF has been studied in educational settings to investigate its role in academic achievement and whether training it may benefit learning. Evidence on the latter is however mixed. Regarding metacognitive training in general, evidence from both disciplines suggests that interventions fostering learners’ self-reflection and knowledge of their learning behaviour (i.e., offline meta-knowledge) may best benefit them and increase academic achievement.

We focused on four aspects of research that could benefit from an interdisciplinary approach between the two areas: (i) validity and reliability of research protocols, (ii) under-researched dimensions of metacognition, (iii) metacognitive training, and (iv) domain-specificity vs. domain generality of metacognitive abilities. To tackle these issue, we propose four avenues for integrated research: (i) investigate the degree to which different protocols relate to similar or different metacognitive constructs, (ii) implement designs and perform experiments to identify neural substrates necessary for offline meta-control by for example borrowing protocols used in educational sciences, (iii) study the effects of (offline) meta-knowledge training on the brain, and (iv) perform developmental research in the metacognitive brain and compare it with the existing developmental literature in educational sciences regarding the domain-generality of metacognitive processes and metacognitive abilities.

First, neurocognitive research on metacognitive judgements has developed robust operationalisations of offline meta-knowledge. However, these operationalisations often consist of specific tasks (e.g., 2-AFC) carried out in the lab. These tasks are often very narrow and do not resemble the challenges and complexities of behaviours associated with learning in schools and universities. Thus, one may question to what extent they reflect real-life metacognition, and to what extent protocols developed in educational sciences and cognitive neuroscience actually operationalise the same components of metacognition. We propose that comparing different protocols from both disciplines that are, a priori, operationalising the same types of metacognitive processes can help evaluate the ecological validity of protocols used in cognitive neuroscience, and allow for more holistic assessments of metacognition, provided that it is clear which protocol assesses which construct. Degrees of correlation between different protocols, within and between disciplines, may allow researchers to assess to what extent they reflect the same metacognitive constructs and also identify what protocols are most appropriate to study a specific construct. For example, a relation between meta- d ′ metacognitive sensitivity in a 2-AFC task and the meta-knowledge subscale of the MAI, would provide external validity to the former. Moreover, educational scientists would be provided with bias-free tools to assess metacognition. These tools may enable researchers to further investigate to what extent metacognitive bias, sensitivity and efficiency each play a role in education settings. In contrast, a low correlation may highlight a difference in domain between the two measures of metacognition. For instance, metacognitive judgements in brain research are made in isolated behaviour, and meta-d’ can thus be viewed to reflect “local” metacognitive sensitivity. It is also unclear to what extent processes involved in these decision-making tasks cover those taking place in a learning environment. When answering self-reported questionnaires, however, subjects make metacognitive judgements on a large set of (learning) activities, and the measures may thus resemble more “global” or domain-general metacognitive sensitivity. In addition, learners in educational settings tend to receive feedback — immediate or delayed — on their learning activities and performance, which is generally not the case for cognitive neuroscience protocols. Therefore, investigating metacognitive judgements in the presence of performance or social feedback may allow researchers to better understand the metacognitive processes at play in educational settings. Devising a global measure of metacognition in the lab by aggregating subjects’ metacognitive abilities in different domains or investigating to what extent local metacognition may affect global metacognition could improve ecological validity significantly. By investigating the neural correlates of educational measures of metacognition, researchers may be able to better understand to what extent the constructs studied in the two disciplines are related. It is indeed possible that, though weakly correlated, the meta-knowledge scale of the MAI and meta-d’ share a common neural basis.

Second, our review highlights gaps in the literature of both disciplines regarding the research of certain types of metacognitive processes. There is a lack of research in offline meta-control (or strategic regulation of cognition) in neuroscience, whereas this construct is widely studied in educational sciences. More specifically, while there exists research on EF related to planning (e.g. 107 ), common experimental designs make it hard to disentangle online from offline metacognitive processes. A few studies have implemented subject reports (e.g., awareness of error or desire for reminders) to pin-point the neural substrates specifically involved in offline meta-control and the current evidence points at a role of the lPFC. More research implementing similar designs may clarify this construct. Alternatively, researchers may exploit educational sciences protocols, such as self-report questionnaires, learning journals, metacognitive prompts and feedback to investigate offline meta-control processes in the brain and their relation to academic proficiency and achievement.

Third, there is only one study known to us on the training of meta-knowledge in the lab 78 . In contrast, meta-knowledge training in educational sciences have been widely studied, in particular with metacognitive prompts and learning journals, although a systematic review would be needed to identify the benefits for learning. Relative to cognitive neuroscience, studies suggest that offline meta-knowledge trained in and outside the lab (i.e., metacognitive judgements and meditation, respectively) transfer to meta-knowledge in other lab tasks. The case of meditation is particularly interesting since meditation has been demonstrated to beneficiate varied aspects of everyday life 108 . Given its importance for efficient regulation of cognition, training (offline) meta-knowledge may present the largest benefits to academic achievement. Hence, it is important to investigate development in the brain relative to meta-knowledge training. Evidence on metacognitive training in educational sciences tends to suggest that offline metacognition is more “plastic” and may therefore benefit learning more than online metacognition. Furthermore, it is important to have a good understanding of the developmental trajectory of metacognitive abilities — not only on a behavioural level but also on a neural level — to identify critical periods for successful training. Doing so would also allow researchers to investigate the potential differences in terms of plasticity that we mention above. Currently, the developmental trajectory of metacognition is under-studied in cognitive neuroscience with only one study that found an overlap between the neural correlates of metacognition in adults and children 109 . On a side note, future research could explore the potential role of genetic factors in metacognitive abilities to better understand to what extent and under what constraints they can be trained.

Fourth, domain-specific and domain-general aspects of metacognitive processes should be further investigated. Educational scientists have studied the development of metacognition in learners and have concluded that metacognitive abilities are domain-specific at the beginning (meaning that their quality depends on the type of learning activity, like mathematics vs. writing) and progressively evolve towards domain-general abilities as knowledge and expertise increase. Similarly, neurocognitive evidence points towards a common network for (offline) metacognitive knowledge which engages the different regions at varying degrees depending on the domain of the activity (i.e., perception, memory, etc.). Investigating this network from a developmental perspective and comparing findings with the existing behavioural literature may improve our understanding of the metacognitive brain and link the two bodies of evidence. It may also enable researchers to identify stages of life more suitable for certain types of metacognitive intervention.

Dunlosky, J. & Metcalfe, J. Metacognition (SAGE Publications, 2008).

Pintrich, P. R. The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing. Theory Into Pract. 41 , 219–225 (2002).

Article   Google Scholar  

Zimmerman, B. J. Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: an overview. Educ. Psychol. 25 , 3–17 (1990).

Zimmerman, B. J. & Schunk, D. H. Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theoretical Perspectives (Routledge, 2001).

Baker, L. & Brown, A. L. Metacognitive Skills and Reading. In Handbook of Reading Research Vol. 1 (ed. Pearson, P. D.) 353–395 (Longman, 1984).

Mckeown, M. G. & Beck, I. L. The role of metacognition in understanding and supporting reading comprehension. In Handbook of Metacognition in Education (eds Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J. & Graesser, A. C.) 19–37 (Routledge, 2009).

Desoete, A., Roeyers, H. & Buysse, A. Metacognition and mathematical problem solving in grade 3. J. Learn. Disabil. 34 , 435–447 (2001).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Veenman, M., Kok, R. & Blöte, A. W. The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills in early adolescence. Instructional Sci. 33 , 193–211 (2005).

Harris, K. R., Graham, S., Brindle, M. & Sandmel, K. Metacognition and children’s writing. In Handbook of metacognition in education 131–153 (Routledge, 2009).

Fleming, S. M. & Dolan, R. J. The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367 , 1338–1349 (2012).

Vaccaro, A. G. & Fleming, S. M. Thinking about thinking: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of metacognitive judgements. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2 , 10.1177%2F2398212818810591 (2018).

Ferrari, M. What can neuroscience bring to education? Educ. Philos. Theory 43 , 31–36 (2011).

Zadina, J. N. The emerging role of educational neuroscience in education reform. Psicol. Educ. 21 , 71–77 (2015).

Meulen, A., van der, Krabbendam, L. & Ruyter, Dde Educational neuroscience: its position, aims and expectations. Br. J. Educ. Stud. 63 , 229–243 (2015).

Varma, S., McCandliss, B. D. & Schwartz, D. L. Scientific and pragmatic challenges for bridging education and neuroscience. Educ. Res. 37 , 140–152 (2008).

van Atteveldt, N., van Kesteren, M. T. R., Braams, B. & Krabbendam, L. Neuroimaging of learning and development: improving ecological validity. Frontline Learn. Res. 6 , 186–203 (2018).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Hruby, G. G. Three requirements for justifying an educational neuroscience. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 82 , 1–23 (2012).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Dignath, C., Buettner, G. & Langfeldt, H.-P. How can primary school students learn self-regulated learning strategies most effectively?: A meta-analysis on self-regulation training programmes. Educ. Res. Rev. 3 , 101–129 (2008).

Jacob, R. & Parkinson, J. The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: a review. Rev. Educ. Res. 85 , 512–552 (2015).

Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z. & Takacs, Z. K. A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near- and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychol. Bull. 145 , 165–188 (2019).

Roebers, C. M. Executive function and metacognition: towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Dev. Rev. 45 , 31–51 (2017).

Clements, D. H., Sarama, J. & Germeroth, C. Learning executive function and early mathematics: directions of causal relations. Early Child. Res. Q. 36 , 79–90 (2016).

Nelson, T. O. & Narens, L. Metamemory. In Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (ed. R. V. Kail & J. W. Hag) 3–33 (Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977).

Baird, J. R. Improving learning through enhanced metacognition: a classroom study. Eur. J. Sci. Educ. 8 , 263–282 (1986).

Flavell, J. H. & Wellman, H. M. Metamemory (1975).

Flavell, J. H. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 34 , 906 (1979).

Livingston, J. A. Metacognition: An Overview. (2003).

Nelson, T. O. Metamemory: a theoretical framework and new findings. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation Vol. 26 (ed. Bower, G. H.) 125–173 (Academic Press, 1990).

Nelson, T. O. & Narens, L. Why investigate metacognition. In Metacognition: Knowing About Knowing (eds Metcalfe, J. & Shimamura, A. P.) 1–25 (MIT Press, 1994).

Shimamura, A. P. A Neurocognitive approach to metacognitive monitoring and control. In Handbook of Metamemory and Memory (eds Dunlosky, J. & Bjork, R. A.) (Routledge, 2014).

Dinsmore, D. L., Alexander, P. A. & Loughlin, S. M. Focusing the conceptual lens on metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 20 , 391–409 (2008).

Borkowski, J. G., Chan, L. K. & Muthukrishna, N. A process-oriented model of metacognition: links between motivation and executive functioning. In (Gregory Schraw & James C. Impara) Issues in the Measurement of Metacognition 1–42 (Buros Institute of Mental Measurements, 2000).

Risko, E. F. & Gilbert, S. J. Cognitive offloading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20 , 676–688 (2016).

Gilbert, S. J. et al. Optimal use of reminders: metacognition, effort, and cognitive offloading. J. Exp. Psychol. 149 , 501 (2020).

Boldt, A. & Gilbert, S. Distinct and overlapping neural correlates of metacognitive monitoring and metacognitive control. Preprint at bioRxiv https://psyarxiv.com/3dz9b/ (2020).

Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A. & Posner, M. I. Executive attention and metacognitive regulation. Conscious Cogn. 9 , 288–307 (2000).

Baker, L., Zeliger-Kandasamy, A. & DeWyngaert, L. U. Neuroimaging evidence of comprehension monitoring. Psihol. teme 23 , 167–187 (2014).

Google Scholar  

Schwartz, B. L. Sources of information in metamemory: Judgments of learning and feelings of knowing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1 , 357–375 (1994).

Nelson, T. O. Metamemory, psychology of. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (eds Smelser, N. J. & Baltes, P. B.) 9733–9738 (Pergamon, 2001).

Hart, J. T. Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. J. Educ. Psychol. 56 , 208 (1965).

Arbuckle, T. Y. & Cuddy, L. L. Discrimination of item strength at time of presentation. J. Exp. Psychol. 81 , 126 (1969).

Fechner, G. T. Elemente der Psychophysik (Breitkopf & Härtel, 1860).

Rouault, M., Seow, T., Gillan, C. M. & Fleming, S. M. Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance. Biol. Psychiatry 84 , 443–451 (2018).

Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J. & Rees, G. Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science 329 , 1541–1543 (2010).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

McCurdy, L. Y. et al. Anatomical coupling between distinct metacognitive systems for memory and visual perception. J. Neurosci. 33 , 1897–1906 (2013).

Fleming, S. M. & Lau, H. C. How to measure metacognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00443 (2014).

Galvin, S. J., Podd, J. V., Drga, V. & Whitmore, J. Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10 , 843–876 (2003).

Metcalfe, J. & Schwartz, B. L. The ghost in the machine: self-reflective consciousness and the neuroscience of metacognition. In (eds Dunlosky, J. & Tauber, S. K.) Oxford Handbook of Metamemory 407–424 (Oxford University Press, 2016).

Maniscalco, B. & Lau, H. A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Conscious Cognition 21 , 422–430 (2012).

Rouault, M., McWilliams, A., Allen, M. G. & Fleming, S. M. Human metacognition across domains: insights from individual differences and neuroimaging. Personal. Neurosci. 1 https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2018.16 (2018).

Rounis, E., Maniscalco, B., Rothwell, J. C., Passingham, R. E. & Lau, H. Theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to the prefrontal cortex impairs metacognitive visual awareness. Cogn. Neurosci. 1 , 165–175 (2010).

Ye, Q., Zou, F., Lau, H., Hu, Y. & Kwok, S. C. Causal evidence for mnemonic metacognition in human precuneus. J. Neurosci. 38 , 6379–6387 (2018).

Fleming, S. M., Huijgen, J. & Dolan, R. J. Prefrontal contributions to metacognition in perceptual decision making. J. Neurosci. 32 , 6117–6125 (2012).

Morales, J., Lau, H. & Fleming, S. M. Domain-general and domain-specific patterns of activity supporting metacognition in human prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 38 , 3534–3546 (2018).

Baird, B., Smallwood, J., Gorgolewski, K. J. & Margulies, D. S. Medial and lateral networks in anterior prefrontal cortex support metacognitive ability for memory and perception. J. Neurosci. 33 , 16657–16665 (2013).

Fleming, S. M., Ryu, J., Golfinos, J. G. & Blackmon, K. E. Domain-specific impairment in metacognitive accuracy following anterior prefrontal lesions. Brain 137 , 2811–2822 (2014).

Baldo, J. V., Shimamura, A. P., Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. & Kaplan, E. Verbal and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 7 , 586–596 (2001).

Froböse, M. I. et al. Catecholaminergic modulation of the avoidance of cognitive control. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147 , 1763 (2018).

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108 , 624 (2001).

Kerns, J. G. et al. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303 , 1023–1026 (2004).

Yeung, N. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. In The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience: The Cutting Edges Vol. 2 (eds Ochsner, K. N. & Kosslyn, S.) 275–299 (Oxford University Press, 2014).

Botvinick, M. M. Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7 , 356–366 (2007).

Fleming, S. M., van der Putten, E. J. & Daw, N. D. Neural mediators of changes of mind about perceptual decisions. Nat. Neurosci. 21 , 617–624 (2018).

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A. & Nieuwenhuis, S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306 , 443–447 (2004).

Koriat, A. The feeling of knowing: some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control. Conscious Cogn. 9 , 149–171 (2000).

Thompson, V. A., Evans, J. & Frankish, K. Dual process theories: a metacognitive perspective. Ariel 137 , 51–43 (2009).

Arango-Muñoz, S. Two levels of metacognition. Philosophia 39 , 71–82 (2011).

Shea, N. et al. Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18 , 186–193 (2014).

Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. & Kok, A. Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology 38 , 752–760 (2001).

Overbeek, T. J., Nieuwenhuis, S. & Ridderinkhof, K. R. Dissociable components of error processing: on the functional significance of the Pe vis-à-vis the ERN/Ne. J. Psychophysiol. 19 , 319–329 (2005).

McGuire, J. T. & Botvinick, M. M. Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107 , 7922–7926 (2010).

Hester, R., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Shpaner, M. & Garavan, H. Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: a comparison of errors made with and without awareness. Neuroimage 27 , 602–608 (2005).

Melby-Lervåg, M. & Hulme, C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 49 , 270 (2013).

Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B. & Laine, M. Working memory training revisited: a multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24 , 1077–1096 (2017).

Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F. & Bühner, M. Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educ. Psychol. 50 , 138–166 (2015).

Karbach, J. & Verhaeghen, P. Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol. Sci. 25 , 2027–2037 (2014).

Patel, R., Spreng, R. N. & Turner, G. R. Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 27 , 187–199 (2013).

Carpenter, J. et al. Domain-general enhancements of metacognitive ability through adaptive training. J. Exp. Psychol. 148 , 51–64 (2019).

Baird, B., Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T. & Schooler, J. W. Domain-specific enhancement of metacognitive ability following meditation training. J. Exp. Psychol. 143 , 1972 (2014).

Winne, P. H. & Perry, N. E. Measuring self-regulated learning. In Handbook of Self-Regulation (eds Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R. & Zeidner, M.) Ch. 16, 531–566 (Academic Press, 2000).

Zimmerman, B. J. & Martinez-Pons, M. Development of a structured interview for assessing student use of self-regulated learning strategies. Am. Educ. Res. J. 23 , 614–628 (1986).

Park, C. Engaging students in the learning process: the learning journal. J. Geogr. High. Educ. 27 , 183–199 (2003).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Harrison, G. M. & Vallin, L. M. Evaluating the metacognitive awareness inventory using empirical factor-structure evidence. Metacogn. Learn. 13 , 15–38 (2018).

Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T. & Mckeachie, W. J. Reliability and predictive validity of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Educ. Psychol. Meas. 53 , 801–813 (1993).

Prevatt, F., Petscher, Y., Proctor, B. E., Hurst, A. & Adams, K. The revised Learning and Study Strategies Inventory: an evaluation of competing models. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66 , 448–458 (2006).

Baggetta, P. & Alexander, P. A. Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 10 , 10–33 (2016).

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C. & Kenworthy, L. Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 6 , 235–238 (2000).

Ohtani, K. & Hisasaka, T. Beyond intelligence: a meta-analytic review of the relationship among metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. Metacogn. Learn. 13 , 179–212 (2018).

Dianovsky, M. T. & Wink, D. J. Student learning through journal writing in a general education chemistry course for pre-elementary education majors. Sci. Educ. 96 , 543–565 (2012).

Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M. & Afflerbach, P. Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacogn Learn. 1 , 3–14 (2006).

Weil, L. G. et al. The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Conscious Cogn. 22 , 264–271 (2013).

Veenman, M. & Spaans, M. A. Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills: Age and task differences. Learn. Individ. Differ. 15 , 159–176 (2005).

Verbert, K. et al. Learning dashboards: an overview and future research opportunities. Personal. Ubiquitous Comput. 18 , 1499–1514 (2014).

Dignath, C. & Büttner, G. Components of fostering self-regulated learning among students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacogn. Learn. 3 , 231–264 (2008).

Hattie, J., Biggs, J. & Purdie, N. Effects of learning skills interventions on student learning: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 66 , 99–136 (1996).

Zohar, A. & Barzilai, S. A review of research on metacognition in science education: current and future directions. Stud. Sci. Educ. 49 , 121–169 (2013).

Berthold, K., Nückles, M. & Renkl, A. Do learning protocols support learning strategies and outcomes? The role of cognitive and metacognitive prompts. Learn. Instr. 17 , 564–577 (2007).

Bannert, M. & Mengelkamp, C. Scaffolding hypermedia learning through metacognitive prompts. In International Handbook of Metacognition and Learning Technologies Vol. 28 (eds Azevedo, R. & Aleven, V.) 171–186 (Springer New York, 2013).

Bannert, M., Sonnenberg, C., Mengelkamp, C. & Pieger, E. Short- and long-term effects of students’ self-directed metacognitive prompts on navigation behavior and learning performance. Comput. Hum. Behav. 52 , 293–306 (2015).

McCrindle, A. R. & Christensen, C. A. The impact of learning journals on metacognitive and cognitive processes and learning performance. Learn. Instr. 5 , 167–185 (1995).

Connor-Greene, P. A. Making connections: evaluating the effectiveness of journal writing in enhancing student learning. Teach. Psychol. 27 , 44–46 (2000).

Wong, B. Y. L., Kuperis, S., Jamieson, D., Keller, L. & Cull-Hewitt, R. Effects of guided journal writing on students’ story understanding. J. Educ. Res. 95 , 179–191 (2002).

Nückles, M., Schwonke, R., Berthold, K. & Renkl, A. The use of public learning diaries in blended learning. J. Educ. Media 29 , 49–66 (2004).

Cantrell, R. J., Fusaro, J. A. & Dougherty, E. A. Exploring the effectiveness of journal writing on learning social studies: a comparative study. Read. Psychol. 21 , 1–11 (2000).

Blair, C. Executive function and early childhood education. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10 , 102–107 (2016).

Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Unlu, F. & Layzer, C. The Efficacy of an Intervention Synthesizing Scaffolding Designed to Promote Self-Regulation with an Early Mathematics Curriculum: Effects on Executive Function (Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2012).

Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S. & Just, M. A. Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia 41 , 1668–1682 (2003).

Sedlmeier, P. et al. The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138 , 1139 (2012).

Bellon, E., Fias, W., Ansari, D. & Smedt, B. D. The neural basis of metacognitive monitoring during arithmetic in the developing brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41 , 4562–4573 (2020).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the University of Amsterdam for supporting this research through the Interdisciplinary Doctorate Agreement grant. W.v.d.B. is further supported by the Jacobs Foundation, European Research Council (grant no. ERC-2018-StG-803338), the European Union Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant no. DiGYMATEX-870578), and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (grant no. NWO-VIDI 016.Vidi.185.068).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Informatics Institute, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Damien S. Fleur & Bert Bredeweg

Departement of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Damien S. Fleur & Wouter van den Bos

Faculty of Education, Amsterdam University of Applied Sciences, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Bert Bredeweg

Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany

Wouter van den Bos

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

D.S.F., B.B. and W.v.d.B. conceived the main conceptual idea of this review article. D.S.F. wrote the manuscript with inputs from and under the supervision of B.B. and W.v.d.B.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Damien S. Fleur .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary materials, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Fleur, D.S., Bredeweg, B. & van den Bos, W. Metacognition: ideas and insights from neuro- and educational sciences. npj Sci. Learn. 6 , 13 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-021-00089-5

Download citation

Received : 06 October 2020

Accepted : 09 April 2021

Published : 08 June 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-021-00089-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Relation of life sciences students’ metacognitive monitoring to neural activity during biology error detection.

  • Mei Grace Behrendt
  • Carrie Clark
  • Joseph Dauer

npj Science of Learning (2024)

The many facets of metacognition: comparing multiple measures of metacognition in healthy individuals

  • Anneke Terneusen
  • Conny Quaedflieg
  • Ieke Winkens

Metacognition and Learning (2024)

Towards a common conceptual space for metacognition in perception and memory

  • Audrey Mazancieux
  • Michael Pereira
  • Céline Souchay

Nature Reviews Psychology (2023)

Predictive Validity of Performance-Based Metacognitive Testing is Superior to Self-report: Evidence from Undergraduate Freshman Students

  • Marcio Alexander Castillo-Diaz
  • Cristiano Mauro Assis Gomes

Trends in Psychology (2023)

Normative data and standardization of an international protocol for the evaluation of metacognition in Spanish-speaking university students: A cross-cultural analysis

  • Antonio P. Gutierrez de Blume
  • Diana Marcela Montoya Londoño
  • Jesus Rivera-Sanchez

Metacognition and Learning (2023)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • Our Mission

Making Metacognition Part of Student Writing

When students are encouraged to think deeply about their writing processes, they become better writers.

High school students writing at their desks

Writing conferences are a staple in many English language arts classrooms today. Teachers recognize the benefit of conversational feedback, allowing students to feel more agency over their own writing, and the power of building rapport that comes with conferences.

In my own classroom, I’ve been on the journey of incorporating writing conferences for over a decade, and they have changed drastically from when I first began. I’ve transitioned from doing most of the talking to students doing more and more sharing. Recently, my thinking on writing conferences has shifted again. After realizing that our conferences were primarily centered on a piece with little to no reflection on the thought process of writing, I added a new layer of complexity. 

Metacognitive Reflection

Metacognition is a term that describes thinking about one’s thinking as a means of reflection. The goal is for students to think more about the process—how they approach writing, barriers to good writing, and strategies that help them write successfully—instead of focusing only on content or rubric requirements. Metacognitive reflection can awaken students to be more aware of their thinking during writing, resulting in a deeper understanding of who they are as writers and of how to transfer their knowledge to any genre of writing. 

So what exactly does metacognitive thinking on writing look like, and how can teachers build this type of reflection into writing conferences?

A whole-class conversation about the importance of metacognition is a good starting place, since students are often focused on assignments rather than their thinking while completing them. These strategies can help students become aware of their thinking while writing and are easy to incorporate in assignments, providing students with opportunities to pause and think about their thinking while writing. Observations from these activities will enable students to talk about metacognition during conferences. 

6 Activities to Encourage Metacognition

1. Keeping a journal. Encourage students to take metacognitive breaks of two to three minutes during writing to record their thoughts. Describe your process to this point . What was a barrier to your writing? How did you overcome this? What do you think you could do to prevent this from occurring next time? These breaks can and should occur at different points in the writing process. 

2. Recording troubleshooting ideas. Encourage students to keep a list of strategies and ideas they have found successful in the past that they can use during writing to help them push through when they’re experiencing difficulty.

3. Writing collaboratively. Provide opportunities for students to work on writing assignments together. The students can discuss why they are making the choices they make along the way. Thoughts can be addressed in comments in a Google Doc or on sticky notes placed on the student’s paper. 

4. Using graphic organizers. Graphic organizers can also serve as tools to guide students to think about their thinking while writing and to identify successful strategies. The object is not to fill the entire graphic organizer but to provide multiple entry points to think about their thinking while writing. 

5. Highlighting papers. I often have students highlight papers for claims, evidence, and analysis, but this can be modified for any focus. This strategy adds a visual component to reflection and opens opportunities for students to think about what leads to strong components of a piece and why other components are weaker.

6. Recording post-writing thoughts. Writing a paragraph on the thought process during an assignment can be particularly helpful for the big-picture process. What would you do differently if writing again? Why? What would you keep the same? Why? What strategies did you employ that worked well that you can use for future writing?

The insights gathered from these metacognitive tools can carry over into writing conversations. In your next writing conferences, try adding some of the italicized questions to questions already commonly asked to gather insight and give input into the thought process behind the writing. 

  • What do you like best about this writing? Why do you think this section is strong? What did you notice as you were writing this section? 
  • Where did you struggle with this piece? Why did you struggle with this section? How did you feel while you were writing this section? What could have helped you while writing this particular section? Let’s review your list of troubleshooting ideas and strategies. What can you add to these?
  • Where is an area you took a risk or experimented with something new? Why did you decide to do something different here? Was it successful? Why or why not? If so, how could you incorporate this into other writing? 
  • How do you feel about the piece overall? How did you feel about the overall process? How do you see yourself growing as a writer? Are there particular things in your learning environment or mindset that contribute to successful writing? Identify one or two concrete strategies to use moving forward. 

Metacognition is an important step in writing instruction and where the real magic happens in learning. Students do need feedback on specific pieces of writing but should be given the opportunity to think beyond the product. Providing students with opportunities for metacognitive reflection and the opportunity to discuss their thinking strengthens their writing not only in class but for years to come.

Center for Teaching

Metacognition.

Thinking about One’s Thinking |   Putting Metacognition into Practice

Thinking about One’s Thinking

metacognitive knowledge essay

Initially studied for its development in young children (Baker & Brown, 1984; Flavell, 1985), researchers soon began to look at how experts display metacognitive thinking and how, then, these thought processes can be taught to novices to improve their learning (Hatano & Inagaki, 1986).  In How People Learn , the National Academy of Sciences’ synthesis of decades of research on the science of learning, one of the three key findings of this work is the effectiveness of a “‘metacognitive’ approach to instruction” (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 2000, p. 18).

Metacognitive practices increase students’ abilities to transfer or adapt their learning to new contexts and tasks (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, p. 12; Palincsar & Brown, 1984; Scardamalia et al., 1984; Schoenfeld, 1983, 1985, 1991).  They do this by gaining a level of awareness above the subject matter : they also think about the tasks and contexts of different learning situations and themselves as learners in these different contexts.  When Pintrich (2002) asserts that “Students who know about the different kinds of strategies for learning, thinking, and problem solving will be more likely to use them” (p. 222), notice the students must “know about” these strategies, not just practice them.  As Zohar and David (2009) explain, there must be a “ conscious meta-strategic level of H[igher] O[rder] T[hinking]” (p. 179).

Metacognitive practices help students become aware of their strengths and weaknesses as learners, writers, readers, test-takers, group members, etc.  A key element is recognizing the limit of one’s knowledge or ability and then figuring out how to expand that knowledge or extend the ability. Those who know their strengths and weaknesses in these areas will be more likely to “actively monitor their learning strategies and resources and assess their readiness for particular tasks and performances” (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, p. 67).

The absence of metacognition connects to the research by Dunning, Johnson, Ehrlinger, and Kruger on “Why People Fail to Recognize Their Own Incompetence” (2003).  They found that “people tend to be blissfully unaware of their incompetence,” lacking “insight about deficiencies in their intellectual and social skills.”  They identified this pattern across domains—from test-taking, writing grammatically, thinking logically, to recognizing humor, to hunters’ knowledge about firearms and medical lab technicians’ knowledge of medical terminology and problem-solving skills (p. 83-84).  In short, “if people lack the skills to produce correct answers, they are also cursed with an inability to know when their answers, or anyone else’s, are right or wrong” (p. 85).  This research suggests that increased metacognitive abilities—to learn specific (and correct) skills, how to recognize them, and how to practice them—is needed in many contexts.

Putting Metacognition into Practice

In “ Promoting Student Metacognition ,” Tanner (2012) offers a handful of specific activities for biology classes, but they can be adapted to any discipline. She first describes four assignments for explicit instruction (p. 116):

  • Preassessments—Encouraging Students to Examine Their Current Thinking: “What do I already know about this topic that could guide my learning?”

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • Retrospective Postassessments—Pushing Students to Recognize Conceptual Change: “Before this course, I thought evolution was… Now I think that evolution is ….” or “How is my thinking changing (or not changing) over time?”
  • Reflective Journals—Providing a Forum in Which Students Monitor Their Own Thinking: “What about my exam preparation worked well that I should remember to do next time? What did not work so well that I should not do next time or that I should change?”

Next are recommendations for developing a “classroom culture grounded in metacognition” (p. 116-118):

  • Giving Students License to Identify Confusions within the Classroom Culture:  ask students what they find confusing, acknowledge the difficulties
  • Integrating Reflection into Credited Course Work: integrate short reflection (oral or written) that ask students what they found challenging or what questions arose during an assignment/exam/project
  • Metacognitive Modeling by the Instructor for Students: model the thinking processes involved in your field and sought in your course by being explicit about “how you start, how you decide what to do first and then next, how you check your work, how you know when you are done” (p. 118)

To facilitate these activities, she also offers three useful tables:

  • Questions for students to ask themselves as they plan, monitor, and evaluate their thinking within four learning contexts—in class, assignments, quizzes/exams, and the course as a whole (p. 115)
  • Prompts for integrating metacognition into discussions of pairs during clicker activities, assignments, and quiz or exam preparation (p. 117)
  • Questions to help faculty metacognitively assess their own teaching (p. 119)

Weimer’s “ Deep Learning vs. Surface Learning: Getting Students to Understand the Difference ” (2012) offers additional recommendations for developing students’ metacognitive awareness and improvement of their study skills:

“[I]t is terribly important that in explicit and concerted ways we make students aware of themselves as learners. We must regularly ask, not only ‘What are you learning?’ but ‘How are you learning?’ We must confront them with the effectiveness (more often ineffectiveness) of their approaches. We must offer alternatives and then challenge students to test the efficacy of those approaches. ” (emphasis added)

She points to a tool developed by Stanger-Hall (2012, p. 297) for her students to identify their study strategies, which she divided into “ cognitively passive ” (“I previewed the reading before class,” “I came to class,” “I read the assigned text,” “I highlighted the text,” et al) and “ cognitively active study behaviors ” (“I asked myself: ‘How does it work?’ and ‘Why does it work this way?’” “I wrote my own study questions,” “I fit all the facts into a bigger picture,” “I closed my notes and tested how much I remembered,” et al) .  The specific focus of Stanger-Hall’s study is tangential to this discussion, 1 but imagine giving students lists like hers adapted to your course and then, after a major assignment, having students discuss which ones worked and which types of behaviors led to higher grades. Even further, follow Lovett’s advice (2013) by assigning “exam wrappers,” which include students reflecting on their previous exam-preparation strategies, assessing those strategies and then looking ahead to the next exam, and writing an action plan for a revised approach to studying. A common assignment in English composition courses is the self-assessment essay in which students apply course criteria to articulate their strengths and weaknesses within single papers or over the course of the semester. These activities can be adapted to assignments other than exams or essays, such as projects, speeches, discussions, and the like.

As these examples illustrate, for students to become more metacognitive, they must be taught the concept and its language explicitly (Pintrich, 2002; Tanner, 2012), though not in a content-delivery model (simply a reading or a lecture) and not in one lesson. Instead, the explicit instruction should be “designed according to a knowledge construction approach,” or students need to recognize, assess, and connect new skills to old ones, “and it needs to take place over an extended period of time” (Zohar & David, p. 187).  This kind of explicit instruction will help students expand or replace existing learning strategies with new and more effective ones, give students a way to talk about learning and thinking, compare strategies with their classmates’ and make more informed choices, and render learning “less opaque to students, rather than being something that happens mysteriously or that some students ‘get’ and learn and others struggle and don’t learn” (Pintrich, 2002, p. 223).

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • What to Expect (when reading philosophy)
  • The Ultimate Goal (of reading philosophy)
  • Basic Good Reading Behaviors
  • Important Background Information, or discipline- and course-specific reading practices, such as “reading for enlightenment” rather than information, and “problem-based classes” rather than historical or figure-based classes
  • A Three-Part Reading Process (pre-reading, understanding, and evaluating)
  • Flagging, or annotating the reading
  • Linear vs. Dialogical Writing (Philosophical writing is rarely straightforward but instead “a monologue that contains a dialogue” [p. 365].)

What would such a handout look like for your discipline?

Students can even be metacognitively prepared (and then prepare themselves) for the overarching learning experiences expected in specific contexts . Salvatori and Donahue’s The Elements (and Pleasures) of Difficulty (2004) encourages students to embrace difficult texts (and tasks) as part of deep learning, rather than an obstacle.  Their “difficulty paper” assignment helps students reflect on and articulate the nature of the difficulty and work through their responses to it (p. 9).  Similarly, in courses with sensitive subject matter, a different kind of learning occurs, one that involves complex emotional responses.  In “ Learning from Their Own Learning: How Metacognitive and Meta-affective Reflections Enhance Learning in Race-Related Courses ” (Chick, Karis, & Kernahan, 2009), students were informed about the common reactions to learning about racial inequality (Helms, 1995; Adams, Bell, & Griffin, 1997; see student handout, Chick, Karis, & Kernahan, p. 23-24) and then regularly wrote about their cognitive and affective responses to specific racialized situations.  The students with the most developed metacognitive and meta-affective practices at the end of the semester were able to “clear the obstacles and move away from” oversimplified thinking about race and racism ”to places of greater questioning, acknowledging the complexities of identity, and redefining the world in racial terms” (p. 14).

Ultimately, metacognition requires students to “externalize mental events” (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, p. 67), such as what it means to learn, awareness of one’s strengths and weaknesses with specific skills or in a given learning context, plan what’s required to accomplish a specific learning goal or activity, identifying and correcting errors, and preparing ahead for learning processes.

————————

1 Students who were tested with short answer in addition to multiple-choice questions on their exams reported more cognitively active behaviors than those tested with just multiple-choice questions, and these active behaviors led to improved performance on the final exam.

  • Adams, Maurianne, Bell, Lee Ann, and Griffin, Pat. (1997). Teaching for diversity and social justice: A sourcebook . New York: Routledge.
  • Bransford, John D., Brown Ann L., and Cocking Rodney R. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school . Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
  • Baker, Linda, and Brown, Ann L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading.  In Paul David Pearson, Michael L. Kamil, Rebecca Barr, & Peter Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of research in reading: Volume III (pp. 353–395).  New York: Longman.
  • Brown, Ann L. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In Rand J. Spiro, Bertram C. Bruce, and William F. Brewer, (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education (pp. 453-482). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Chick, Nancy, Karis, Terri, and Kernahan, Cyndi. (2009). Learning from their own learning: how metacognitive and meta-affective reflections enhance learning in race-related courses . International Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 3(1). 1-28.
  • Commander, Nannette Evans, and Valeri-Gold, Marie. (2001). The learning portfolio: A valuable tool for increasing metacognitive awareness . The Learning Assistance Review, 6 (2), 5-18.
  • Concepción, David. (2004). Reading philosophy with background knowledge and metacognition . Teaching Philosophy , 27 (4). 351-368.
  • Dunning, David, Johnson, Kerri, Ehrlinger, Joyce, and Kruger, Justin. (2003) Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence . Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12 (3). 83-87.
  • Flavell,  John H. (1985). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Hatano, Giyoo and Inagaki, Kayoko. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In Harold Stevenson, Azuma, Horishi, and Hakuta, Kinji (Eds.), Child development and education in Japan, New York: W.H. Freeman.
  • Helms, Janet E. (1995). An update of Helms’ white and people of color racial identity models . In J.G. Ponterotto, Joseph G., Casas, Manuel, Suzuki, Lisa A., and Alexander, Charlene M. (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 181-198) . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Lovett, Marsha C. (2013). Make exams worth more than the grade. In Matthew Kaplan, Naomi Silver, Danielle LaVague-Manty, and Deborah Meizlish (Eds.), Using reflection and metacognition to improve student learning: Across the disciplines, across the academy . Sterling, VA: Stylus.
  • Palincsar, Annemarie Sullivan, and Brown, Ann L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities . Cognition and Instruction, 1 (2). 117-175.
  • Pintrich, Paul R. (2002). The Role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing . Theory into Practice, 41 (4). 219-225.
  • Salvatori, Mariolina Rizzi, and Donahue, Patricia. (2004). The Elements (and pleasures) of difficulty . New York: Pearson-Longman.
  • Scardamalia, Marlene, Bereiter, Carl, and Steinbach, Rosanne. (1984). Teachability of reflective processes in written composition . Cognitive Science , 8, 173-190.
  • Schoenfeld, Alan H. (1991). On mathematics as sense making: An informal attack on the fortunate divorce of formal and informal mathematics. In James F. Voss, David N. Perkins, and Judith W. Segal (Eds.), Informal reasoning and education (pp. 311-344). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Stanger-Hall, Kathrin F. (2012). Multiple-choice exams: An obstacle for higher-level thinking in introductory science classes . Cell Biology Education—Life Sciences Education, 11(3), 294-306.
  • Tanner, Kimberly D.  (2012). Promoting student metacognition . CBE—Life Sciences Education, 11, 113-120.
  • Weimer, Maryellen.  (2012, November 19). Deep learning vs. surface learning: Getting students to understand the difference . Retrieved from the Teaching Professor Blog from http://www.facultyfocus.com/articles/teaching-professor-blog/deep-learning-vs-surface-learning-getting-students-to-understand-the-difference/ .
  • Zohar, Anat, and David, Adi Ben. (2009). Paving a clear path in a thick forest: a conceptual analysis of a metacognitive component . Metacognition Learning , 4 , 177-195.

Creative Commons License

Photo credit:  wittygrittyinvisiblegirl via  Compfight cc

Photo Credit: Helga Weber via Compfight cc

Photo Credit: fiddle oak via Compfight cc

Teaching Guides

  • Online Course Development Resources
  • Principles & Frameworks
  • Pedagogies & Strategies
  • Reflecting & Assessing
  • Challenges & Opportunities
  • Populations & Contexts

Quick Links

  • Services for Departments and Schools
  • Examples of Online Instructional Modules

Metacognitive writing strategies, critical thinking skills, and academic writing performance: A structural equation modeling approach

  • Published: 24 November 2022
  • Volume 18 , pages 237–260, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • Mark Feng Teng   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5134-8504 1 &
  • Mei Yue   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-0688-1040 2  

2335 Accesses

16 Citations

1 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The present study adopted the structural equation modeling approach to examine Chinese university students’ metacognition, critical thinking skills, and academic writing. In particular, this research explored whether awareness in metacognition can foster critical thinking and, thus, lead to enhancement in academic writing. The measure for exploring metacognitive writing strategies covered metacognitive knowledge and regulation in academic writing. The measure for understanding learners’ critical thinking encompassed the following five skills: inference, recognition of assumptions, deduction, interpretations, and evaluation of arguments. The academic writing assessment was based on an internal test. The participants consisted of 644 third-year students from a Chinese university. Three models tested: (1) the role of metacognition in academic writing; (2) the role of metacognition in critical thinking; and (3) correlations between metacognition, critical thinking skills, and academic writing. The results indicated significant relationships between the three variables, and the implications based on these findings were discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

metacognitive knowledge essay

Similar content being viewed by others

metacognitive knowledge essay

Validation of metacognitive academic writing strategies and the predictive effects on academic writing performance in a foreign language context

metacognitive knowledge essay

Metacognition in Academic Writing: Learning Dimensions

metacognitive knowledge essay

A path model for metacognition and its relation to problem-solving strategies and achievement for different tasks

Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quartely, 31 , 71–94.

Atkinson, D., & Ramanathan, V. (1995). Cultures of writing: An ethnographic comparison of Ll and L2 university writing language programs. TESOL Quarterly, 29 , 539–568.

Article   Google Scholar  

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition . Erlbaum.

Google Scholar  

Bernard, R. M., Zhang, D., Abrami, P. C., Sicoly, F., Borokhovski, E., & Surkes, M. A. (2008). Exploring the structure of the watson–glaser critical thinking appraisal: One scale or many subscales? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3 , 15–22.

Bui, G., & Kong, A. (2019). Metacognitive instruction for peer review interaction in L2 writing. Journal of Writing Research, 11 (2), 357–392.

Camp, H. (2012). The psychology of writing development—and its implications for assessment. Assessing Writing, 17 (2), 92–105.

Chaffee, J. (2015). Critical thinking, thoughtful writing (6th ed.). Cengage Learning.

Cottrell, S. (2017). Critical thinking skills: Effective analysis, argument and reflection . Bloomsbury Publishing.

Book   Google Scholar  

D’Alessio, F., Avolio, B., & Charles, V. (2019). Studying the impact of critical thinking on the academic performance of executive MBA students. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 31 , 275–283.

De Silva, R., & Graham, S. (2015). The effects of strategy instruction on writing strategy use for students of different proficiency levels. System, 53 , 47–59.

Efklides, A. (2001). Metacognitive experiences in problem solving: Metacognition, motivation, and self-regulation. In A. Efklides, J. Kuhl, & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 297–323). Kluwer.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Efklides, A. (2006). Metacognition and affect: What can metacognitive experiences tell us about the learning process? Educational Research Review, 1 , 3–14.

Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: Defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation. European Psychologist, 13 , 277–287.

Efklides, A., Niemivirta, M., & Yamauchi, H. (2003). Motivation and self-regulation: Processes involved and context effects—A discussion. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 46 , 38–52.

Evans, J St. .B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8 , 223–241.

Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new era of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34 , 906–911.

Flavell, J. H. (2004). Theory-of-Mind development: Retrospect and prospect. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50 , 274–290.

Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The dynamics of composing: Making plans and juggling constraints. In L. Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 31–50). Erlbaum.

Gammil, D. (2006). Learning the write way. The Reading Teacher, 59 (8), 754–762.

Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educational Psychologist, 35 , 3–12.

Graham, S. (2006). Writing. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.,), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 457–478). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hacker, D. J., Keener, M. C., & Kircher, J. C. (2009). Writing is applied metacognition. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 154–172). Routledge.

Hall, N. C., & Goetz, T. (Eds.). (2013). Emotion, motivation, and self-regulation: A handbook for teachers . Bingley.

Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53 (4), 449–455.

Harris, K. R., Santangelo, T., & Graham, S. (2010). Metacognition and strategies instruction in writing. In H. S. Waters & W. Schneider (Eds.), Metacognition, strategy use, and instruction (pp. 226–256). The Guilford Press.

Hedgcock, J. S. (2012). Second language writing processes among adolescent and adult learners. In E. L. Grigorenko, E. Mambrino, & D. D. Preiss (Eds.), Writing a mosaic of new perspectives (pp. 221–239). Psychology Press.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6 , 1–55.

Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children's metacognition about reading: Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22 (3-4), 255–278.

Kellogg, R. T. (1994). The psychology of writing . Oxford University Press.

Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: A cognitive developmental perspective. Journal of Writing Research, 1 (1), 1–26.

Kline, R. B. (1998). Methodology in the social sciences. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling . Guilford Press.

Ku, K. Y. L., & Ho, I. T. (2010). Metacognitive strategies that enhance critical thinking. Metacognition and Learning, 5 , 251–267.

Magno, C. (2010). The role of metacognitive skills in developing critical thinking. Metacognition and Learning, 5 , 137–156.

Mahoney, B. (2011). Critical thinking in psychology: Personality and individual differences . Learning Matters.

McCormick, C. B. (2003). Metacognition and learning. In W. M. Reynolds & G. E. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 79–102). John Wiley & Sons.

Mehta, S. R., & Al-Mahrooqi, R. (2015). Can thinking be taught? Linking critical thinking and writing in an EFL context. RELC Journal, 46 , 23–36.

Meijera, J., Sleegersb, P., Elshout-Mohra, M., van Daalen-Kapteijnsa, M., Meeusc, W., & Tempelaar, D. (2013). The development of a questionnaire on metacognition for students in higher education. Education Research, 55 , 31–52.

Moss, P. A., & Kozdiol, S. M. (1991). Investigating the validity of a locally developed critical thinking test. Educational Measurement Issues and Practice, 10 (3), 17–22.

Negretti, R., & McGrath, L. (2018). Scaffolding genre knowledge and metacognition: Insights from an L2 doctoral research writing course. Journal of Second Language Writing, 40 , 12–31.

Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51 , 102–116.

Newman, D. R., Webb, B., & Cochrane, C. (1995). A content analysis method to measure critical thinking in face-to-face and computer supported group learning. Interpersonal Computing and Technology, 3 (2), 56–77.

Oxford, R. L. (2013). Teaching and researching language learning strategies (2nd ed.). Pearson.

Ozarska, M. (2008). Some suggestions for academic writing instruction at English teacher training colleges. English Teaching Forum, 48 , 30–33.

Pally, M. (2001). Skills development in 'sustained' contentbased curricula: Case studies in analytical/critical thinking and academic writing. Language and Education, 15 (4), 279–305.

Paul, R. (1995). What every student needs to survive in a rapidly changing world . The Foundation for Critical Thinking.

Phakiti, A. (2018). Assessing higher-order thinking skills in language learning. In J. I. Liontas (Ed.), The TESOL encyclopedia of English language teaching (pp. 1–7). Wiley.

Pintrich, P. (2002). The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing. Theory into Practice, 41 (4), 219–225.

Pintrich, P. R., Wolters, C. A., & Baxter, G. P. (2000). Assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning. In J. C. Impara, G. Schraw, & J. C. Impara (Eds.), Issues in the measurement of metacognition (pp. 43–97). University of Nebraska-Lincoln.

Puustinen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (2001). Models of Self-regulated learning: A review. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 45 , 269–286.

Qin, L. M., & Zhang, L. J. (2019). English as a foreign language writers’ metacognitive strategy knowledge of writing and their working performance in multimedia environments. Journal of Writing Research, 12 (2), 393–413.

Ramanathan, V., & Atkinson, D. (1999). Individualism, academic writing, and ESL writers. Journal of Second Language Writing, 8 , 45–75.

Ruan, Z. (2014). Metacognitive awareness of EFL student writers in a Chinese ELT context. Language Awareness, 23 (1–2), 76–91.

Sato, M. (2022). Metacognition. In S. Li, P. Hiver & M. Papi (eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and individual differences (95–108). Routledge.

Schoenfeld, A. (1987). What's all the fuss about metacognition. In Schoen- feld, A. (Ed.) Cognitive science and mathematics education (pp.189–215). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Schraw, G. A. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instructional Science, 26 , 113–125.

Schraw, G. A. (2001). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. In H. J. Hartman (Ed.), Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory, research and practice (pp. 3–16). Springer.

Schraw, G., & Dennison, R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19 , 460–475.

Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories. Educational Psychology Review, 7 (4), 351–371.

Schroyens, W. (2005). Knowledge and thought: An introduction to critical thinking. Experimental Psychology, 52 (2), 163–164.

Schuster, S. (2019). The critical thinker: The path to better problem solving, accurate decision making, and self-disciplined thinking . CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform.

Shimamura, A. P. (2000). Toward a cognitive neuroscience of metacognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 9 , 313–323.

Shor, I. (1992). Empowering education: Critical teaching for social change . University of Chicago Press.

Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning . Elrbaum.

Stanovich, K. E., & Toplak, M. E. (2012). Defining features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type 2 processing. Mind & Society, 11 , 3–13.

Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Distinguishing the reflective, algorithmic, and autonomous minds: Is it time for a tri-process theory? In J. St. B. T. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond (pp.55–88). New York: Oxford University Press.

Sternberg, R. (1985). Approaches to intelligence. In S. F. Chipman, J. W. Segal & R. Glaser, (Eds.) Thinking and learning skills (Vol.2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tan, M., Randi, J., Barbot, B., Levenson, C., Friedlaender, L. K., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2012). Seeing, connecting, writing: Developing creativity and narrative writing in children. In E. L. Grigorenko, E. Mambrino, & D. Preiss (Eds.), Writing: A mosaic of new perspectives (pp. 275–291). Psychology Press.

Teng, F. (2016). Immediate and delayed effects of embedded metacognitive instruction on Chinese EFL students’ English writing and regulation of cognition. Thinking Skills & Creativity, 22 , 289–302.

Teng, F. (2019). The role of metacognitive knowledge and regulation in mediating university EFL learners’ writing performance. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching . https://doi.org/10.1080/17501229.2019.1615493

Teng, F. (2021). Interactive-whiteboard-technology-supported collaborative writing: Writing achievement, metacognitive activities, and co-regulation patterns. System, 97 , 102426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2020.102426

Teng, F., & Huang, J. (2019). Predictive effects of writing strategies for self-regulated learning on secondary school learners’ EFL writing proficiency. TESOL Quarterly, 53 , 232–247.

Teng, F., & Huang, J. (2021). The effects of incorporating metacognitive strategies instruction into collaborative writing on writing complexity, accuracy, and fluency. Asia Pacific Journal of Education . https://doi.org/10.1080/02188791.2021.1982675

Teng, L. S., & Zhang, L. J. (2018). Effects of motivational regulation strategies on writing performance: A mediation model of self-regulated learning of writing in English as a second/foreign language. Metacognition and Learning, 13 , 213–240.

Teng, F., & Zhang, L. J. (2021). Development of children’s metacognitive knowledge, and reading and writing proficiency in English as a foreign language: Longitudinal data using multilevel models. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 91 (4), 1202–1230.

Teng, F., Wang, C., & Zhang, L. J. (2022a). Assessing self-regulatory writing strategies and their predictive effects on young EFL learners’ writing performance. Assessing Writing, 51 , 100573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asw.2021.100573

Teng, F., Qin, C., & Wang, C. (2022b). Validation of metacognitive academic writing strategies and the predictive effects on academic writing performance in a foreign language context. Metacognition and Learning, 17 , 167–190.

Troia, G. A., Harbaugh, A. G., Shankland, R. K., Wolbers, K. A., & Lawrence, A. M. (2013). Relationships between writing motivation, writing activity, and writing performance: Effects of grade, sex, and ability. Reading and Writing, 26 , 17–44.

Veenman, M. V. J., & Elshout, J. J. (1999). Changes in the relation between cognitive and metacognitive skills during the acquisition of expertise. European Journal of Psychology of Education, XIV , 509–523.

Veenman, M. V. J., & Van Cleef, D. (2019). Measuring metacognitive skills for mathematics: Students’ self-reports vs. on-line assessment methods. ZDM International Journal on Mathematics Education, 51 , 691–701.

Veenman, M. V. J., van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1 , 3–14.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R.W. Rieber & A.S. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general psychology (pp. 39–285). Plenum Press.

Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1980). Watson-Glaser critical thinking appraisal . Psychological Corp.

Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (2008). Watson-glaser critical thinking appraisal: Short form manual . Pearson.

Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (2009). Watson-glaser II critical thinking appraisal: Technical manual and user’s guide . Pearson.

Wenden, A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language learning. Applied Linguistics, 19 , 515–537.

Wenden, A. (2001). Metacognitive knowledge in SLA: The neglected variable. In M. Breen (Ed.), Learner contributions to language learning: New directions in research (pp. 44–64). Harlow: Pearson Education

Wolters, C. A. (1999). The relation between high school students’ motivational regulation and their use of learning strategies, effort, and classroom performance. Learning & Individual Differences, 11 , 281–299.

Wolters, C. A., & Benzon, M. B. (2013). Assessing and predicting college students’ use of strategies for the self-regulation of motivation. Journal of Experimental Education, 81 , 199–221.

Woodrow, L. (2011). College English writing affect: Self efficacy and anxiety. System, 39 , 510–522.

Zhang, L. J., & Qin, T. L. (2018). Validating a questionnaire on EFL writers’ metacognitive awareness of writing strategies in multimedia environments. In A. Haukås, C. Bjørke, & Dypedahl, M. (Eds.), Metacognition in language learning and teaching (pp. 157–179). London, England: Routledge.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An overview. Theory into Practice, 41 (2), 64–70.

Zimmerman, B. J., & Risemberg, R. (1997). Becoming a self-regulated writer: A social cognitive perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22 , 73–101.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by the Project from the Education Department of Hainan Province (Project number: Hnky2020ZD-9). We appreciate Professor Chuang Wang’ help in proofreading this article.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Center for Linguistic Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Zhuhai, China

Mark Feng Teng

School of Foreign Languages, Hainan University, Haikou, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mei Yue .

Ethics declarations

This article involves human participants performed by the authors. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants involved in the study. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (PDF 365 kb)

Rights and permissions.

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Teng, M.F., Yue, M. Metacognitive writing strategies, critical thinking skills, and academic writing performance: A structural equation modeling approach. Metacognition Learning 18 , 237–260 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09328-5

Download citation

Received : 27 March 2022

Accepted : 07 November 2022

Published : 24 November 2022

Issue Date : April 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09328-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Critical thinking
  • Metacognitive knowledge
  • Metacognitive regulation
  • Metacognition
  • Academic writing
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Change Password

Your password must have 8 characters or more and contain 3 of the following:.

  • a lower case character, 
  • an upper case character, 
  • a special character 

Password Changed Successfully

Your password has been changed

  • Sign in / Register

Request Username

Can't sign in? Forgot your username?

Enter your email address below and we will send you your username

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to retrieve your username

Fostering Metacognition to Support Student Learning and Performance

  • Julie Dangremond Stanton
  • Amanda J. Sebesta
  • John Dunlosky

*Address correspondence to: Julie Dangremond Stanton ( E-mail Address: [email protected] ).

Department of Cellular Biology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602

Search for more papers by this author

Department of Biology, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO 63103

Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH 44240

Metacognition is awareness and control of thinking for learning. Strong metacognitive skills have the power to impact student learning and performance. While metacognition can develop over time with practice, many students struggle to meaningfully engage in metacognitive processes. In an evidence-based teaching guide associated with this paper ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student-metacognition ), we outline the reasons metacognition is critical for learning and summarize relevant research on this topic. We focus on three main areas in which faculty can foster students’ metacognition: supporting student learning strategies (i.e., study skills), encouraging monitoring and control of learning, and promoting social metacognition during group work. We distill insights from key papers into general recommendations for instruction, as well as a special list of four recommendations that instructors can implement in any course. We encourage both instructors and researchers to target metacognition to help students improve their learning and performance.

INTRODUCTION

Supporting the development of metacognition is a powerful way to promote student success in college. Students with strong metacognitive skills are positioned to learn more and perform better than peers who are still developing their metacognition (e.g., Wang et al. , 1990 ). Students with well-developed metacognition can identify concepts they do not understand and select appropriate strategies for learning those concepts. They know how to implement strategies they have selected and carry out their overall study plans. They can evaluate their strategies and adjust their plans based on outcomes. Metacognition allows students to be more expert-like in their thinking and more effective and efficient in their learning. While collaborating in small groups, students can also stimulate metacognition in one another, leading to improved outcomes. Ever since metacognition was first described ( Flavell, 1979 ), enthusiasm for its potential impact on student learning has remained high. In fact, as of today, the most highly cited paper in CBE—Life Sciences Education is an essay on “Promoting Student Metacognition” ( Tanner, 2012 ).

Despite this enthusiasm, instructors face several challenges when attempting to harness metacognition to improve their students’ learning and performance. First, metacognition is a term that has been used so broadly that its meaning may not be clear ( Veenman et al. , 2006 ). We define metacognition as awareness and control of thinking for learning ( Cross and Paris, 1988 ). Metacognition includes metacognitive knowledge , which is your awareness of your own thinking and approaches for learning. Metacognition also includes metacognitive regulation , which is how you control your thinking for learning ( Figure 1 ). Second, metacognition includes multiple processes and skills that are named and emphasized differently in the literature from various disciplines. Yet upon examination, the metacognitive processes and skills from different fields are closely related, and they often overlap (see Supplemental Figure 1). Third, metacognition consists of a person’s thoughts, which may be challenging for that person to describe. The tacit nature of metacognitive processes makes it difficult for instructors to observe metacognition in their students, and it also makes metacognition difficult for researchers to measure. As a result, classroom intervention studies of metacognition—those that are necessary for making the most confident recommendations for promoting student metacognition—have lagged behind foundational and laboratory research on metacognitive processes and skills.

FIGURE 1. Metacognition framework commonly used in biology education research (modified from Schraw and Moshman, 1995 ). This theoretical framework divides metacognition into two components: metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive regulation. Metacognitive knowledge includes what you know about your own thinking and what you know about strategies for learning. Declarative knowledge involves knowing about yourself as a learner, the demands of the task, and what learning strategies exist. Procedural knowledge involves knowing how to use learning strategies. Conditional knowledge involves knowing when and why to use particular learning strategies. Metacognitive regulation involves the actions you take in order to learn. Planning involves deciding what strategies to use for a future learning task and when you will use them. Monitoring involves assessing your understanding of concepts and the effectiveness of your strategies while learning. Evaluating involves appraising your prior plan and adjusting it for future learning.

How do undergraduate students develop metacognitive skills?

To what extent do active learning and generative work 1 promote metacognition?

To what extent do increases in metacognition correspond to increases in achievement in science courses?

FIGURE 2. (A) Landing page for the Student Metacognition guide. The landing page provides a map with sections an instructor can click on to learn more about how to support students’ metacognition. (B) Example paper summary showing instructor recommendations. At the end of each summary in our guide, we used italicized text to point out what instructors should know based on the paper’s results.

The organization of this essay reflects the organization of our evidence-based teaching guide. In the guide, we first define terms and provide important background from papers that highlight the underpinnings and benefits of metacognition ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student-metacognition/benefits-definitions-underpinnings ). We then explore metacognition research by summarizing both classic and recent papers in the field and providing links for readers who want to examine the original studies. We consider three main areas related to metacognition: 1) student strategies for learning, 2) monitoring and control of learning, and 3) social metacognition during group work.

SUPPORTING STUDENTS TO USE EFFECTIVE LEARNING STRATEGIES

What strategies do students use for learning.

First our teaching guide examines metacognition in the context of independent study ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student-metacognition/supporting-student
-learning-strategies ). When students transition to college, they have increased responsibility for directing their learning, which includes making important decisions about how and when to study. Students rely on their metacognition to make those decisions, and they also use metacognitive processes and skills while studying on their own. Empirical work has confirmed what instructors observe about their own students’ studying—many students rely on passive strategies for learning. Students focus on reviewing material as it is written or presented, as opposed to connecting concepts and synthesizing information to make meaning. Some students use approaches that engage their metacognition, but they often do so without a full understanding of the benefits of these approaches ( Karpicke et al. , 2009 ). Students also tend to study based on exam dates and deadlines, rather than planning out when to study ( Hartwig and Dunlosky, 2012 ). As a result, they tend to cram, which is also known in the literature as massing their study. Students continue to cram because this approach is often effective for boosting short-term performance, although it does not promote long-term retention of information.

Which Strategies Should Students Use for Learning?

Here, we make recommendations about what students should do to learn, as opposed to what they typically do. In our teaching guide, we highlight three of the most effective strategies for learning: 1) self-testing, 2) spacing, and 3) interleaving ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student
-metacognition/supporting-student-learning-strategies/
#whatstudentsshould ). These strategies are not yet part of many students’ metacognitive knowledge, but they should know about them and be encouraged to use them while metacognitively regulating their learning. Students self-test when they use flash cards and answer practice questions in an attempt to recall information. Self-testing provides students with opportunities to monitor their understanding of material and identify gaps in their understanding. Self-testing also allows students to activate relevant knowledge and encode prompted information so it can be more easily accessed from their memory in the future ( Dunlosky et al. , 2013 ).

Students space their studying when they spread their learning of the same material over multiple sessions. This approach requires students to intentionally plan their learning instead of focusing only on what is “due” next. Spacing can be combined with retrieval practice , which involves recalling information from memory. For example, self-testing is a form of retrieval practice. Retrieval practice with spacing encourages students to actively recall the same content across several study sessions, which is essential for consolidating information from prior study periods ( Dunlosky et al. , 2013 ). Importantly, when students spread their learning over multiple sessions, they are less susceptible to superficial familiarity with concepts, which can mislead them into thinking they have learned concepts based on recognition alone ( Kornell and Bjork, 2008 ).

Students interleave when they alternate studying of information from one category with studying of information from another category. For example, when students learn categories of amino acid side groups, they should alternate studying nonpolar amino acids with polar amino acids. This allows students to discriminate across categories, which is often critical for correctly solving problems ( Rohrer et al. , 2020 ). Interleaving between categories also supports student learning because it usually results in spacing of study.

How are students enacting specific learning strategies, and do different students enact them in different ways?

To what extent do self-testing, spacing, and interleaving support achievement in the context of undergraduate science courses?

What can instructors do to increase students’ use of effective learning strategies?

What Factors Affect the Strategies Students Should Use to Learn?

Next, we examined the factors that affect what students should do to learn. Although we recommend three well-established strategies for learning, other appropriate strategies can vary based on the learning context. For example, the nature of the material, the type of assessment, the learning objectives, and the instructional methods can render some strategies more effective than others ( Scouller, 1998 ; Sebesta and Bray Speth, 2017 ). Strategies for learning can be characterized as deep if they involve extending and connecting ideas or applying knowledge and skills in new ways ( Baeten et al. , 2010 ). Strategies can be characterized as surface if they involve recalling and reproducing content. While surface strategies are often viewed negatively, there are times when these approaches can be effective for learning ( Hattie and Donoghue, 2016 ). For example, when students have not yet gained background knowledge in an area, they can use surface strategies to acquire the necessary background knowledge. They can then incorporate deep strategies to extend, connect, and apply this knowledge. Importantly, surface and deep strategies can be used simultaneously for effective learning. The use of surface and deep strategies ultimately depends on what students are expected to know and be able to do, and these expectations are set by instructors. Openly discussing these expectations with students can enable them to more readily select effective strategies for learning.

What Challenges Do Students Face in Using Their Metacognition to Enact Effective Strategies?

How can students address challenges they will face when using effective—but effortful—strategies for learning?

What approaches can instructors take to help students overcome these challenges?

ENCOURAGING STUDENTS TO MONITOR AND CONTROL THEIR LEARNING FOR EXAMS

Metacognition can be investigated in the context of any learning task, but in the sciences, metacognitive processes and skills are most often investigated in the context of high-stakes exams. Because exams are a form of assessment common to nearly every science course, in the next part of our teaching guide, we summarized some of the vast research focused on monitoring and control before, during, and after an exam ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student-metacognition/encouraging-students-monitor-control-learning ). In the following section, we demonstrate the kinds of monitoring and control decisions learners make by using an example of introductory biology students studying for an exam on cell division. The students’ instructor has explained that the exam will focus on the stages of mitosis and cytokinesis, and the exam will include both multiple-choice and short-answer questions.

How Should Students Use Metacognition while Preparing for and Taking an Exam?

As students prepare for an exam, they can use metacognition to inform their learning. Students can consider how they will be tested, set goals for their learning, and make a plan to meet their goals. It is expected that students who set specific goals while planning for an exam will be more effective in their studying than students who do not make specific goals. For example, a student who sets a specific goal to identify areas of confusion each week by answering end-of-chapter questions each weekend is expected to do better than a student who sets a more general goal of staying up-to-date on the material. Although some studies include goal setting and planning as one of many metacognitive strategies introduced to students, the influence of task-specific goal setting on academic achievement has not been well studied on its own in the context of science courses.

As students study, it is critical that they monitor both their use of learning strategies and their understanding of concepts. Yet many students struggle to accurately monitor their own understanding ( de Carvalho Filho, 2009 ). In the example we are considering, students may believe they have already learned mitosis because they recognize the terms “prophase,” “metaphase,” “anaphase,” and “telophase” from high school biology. When students read about mitosis in the textbook, processes involving the mitotic spindle may seem familiar because of their exposure to these concepts in class. As a result, students may inaccurately predict that they will perform well on exam questions focused on the mitotic spindle, and their overconfidence may cause them to stop studying the mitotic spindle and related processes ( Thiede et al. , 2003 ). Students often rate their confidence in their learning based on their ability to recognize, rather than recall, concepts.

Instead of focusing on familiarity, students should rate their confidence based on how well they can retrieve relevant information to correctly answer questions. Opportunities for practicing retrieval, such as self-testing, can improve monitoring accuracy. Instructors can help students monitor their understanding more accurately by encouraging students to complete practice exams and giving students feedback on their answers, perhaps in the form of a key or a class discussion ( Rawson and Dunlosky, 2007 ). Returning to the example, if students find they can easily recall the information needed to correctly answer questions about cytokinesis, they may wisely decide to spend their study time on other concepts. In contrast, if students struggle to remember information needed to answer questions about the mitotic spindle, and they answer these questions incorrectly, then they can use this feedback to direct their efforts toward mastering the structure and function of the mitotic spindle.

While taking a high-stakes exam, students can again monitor their performance on a single question, a set of questions, or an entire exam. Their monitoring informs whether they change an answer, with students tending to change answers they judge as incorrect. Accordingly, the accuracy of their monitoring will influence whether their changes result in increased performance ( Koriat and Goldsmith, 1996 ). In some studies, changing answers on an exam has been shown to increase student performance, in contrast to the common belief that a student’s first answer is usually right ( Stylianou-Georgiou and Papanastasiou, 2017 ). Changing answers on an exam can be beneficial if students return to questions they had low confidence in answering and make a judgment on their answers based on the ability to retrieve the information from memory, rather than a sense of familiarity with the concepts. Two important open questions are:

What techniques can students use to improve the accuracy of their monitoring, while preparing for an exam and while taking an exam?

How often do students monitor their understanding when studying on their own?

How Should Students Use Metacognition after Taking an Exam?

How do students develop metacognitive regulation skills such as evaluation?

To what extent does the ability to evaluate affect student learning and performance?

When students evaluate the outcome of their studying and believe their preparation was lacking, to what degree do they adopt more effective strategies for the next exam?

PROMOTING SOCIAL METACOGNITION DURING GROUP WORK

Next, our teaching guide covers a relatively new area of inquiry in the field of metacognition called social metacognition , which is also known as socially shared metacognition ( https://lse.ascb.org/evidence-based-teaching-guides/student
-metacognition/promoting-social-metacognition
-group-work ). Science students are expected to learn not only on their own, but also in the context of small groups. Understanding social metacognition is important because it can support effective student learning during collaborations both inside and outside the classroom. While individual metacognition involves awareness and control of one’s own thinking, social metacognition involves awareness and control of others’ thinking. For example, social metacognition happens when students share ideas with peers, invite peers to evaluate their ideas, and evaluate ideas shared by peers ( Goos et al. , 2002 ). Students also use social metacognition when they assess, modify, and enact one another’s strategies for solving problems ( Van De Bogart et al. , 2017 ). While enacting problem-solving strategies, students can evaluate their peers’ hypotheses, predictions, explanations, and interpretations. Importantly, metacognition and social metacognition are expected to positively affect one another ( Chiu and Kuo, 2009 ).

How do social metacognition and individual metacognition affect one another?

How can science instructors help students to effectively use social metacognition during group work?

CONCLUSIONS

We encourage instructors to support students’ success by helping them develop their metacognition. Our teaching guide ends with an Instructor Checklist of actions instructors can take to include opportunities for metacognitive practice in their courses ( https://lse.ascb.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/10/2020/12/Student-Metacognition-Instructor-Checklist.pdf ). We also provide a list of the most promising approaches instructors can take, called Four Strategies to Implement in Any Course ( https://lse.ascb.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/10/2020/12/Four
-Strategies-to-Foster-Student-Metacognition.pdf ). We not only encourage instructors to consider using these strategies, but given that more evidence for their efficacy is needed from classroom investigations, we also encourage instructors to evaluate and report how well these strategies are improving their students’ achievement. By exploring and supporting students’ metacognitive development, we can help them learn more and perform better in our courses, which will enable them to develop into lifelong learners.

1 Generative work “involves students working individually or collaboratively to generate ideas and products that go beyond what has been presented to them” ( Andrews et al. , 2019 , p2). Generative work is often stimulated by active-learning approaches.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are grateful to Cynthia Brame, Kristy Wilson, and Adele Wolfson for their insightful feedback on this paper and the guide. This material is based upon work supported in part by the National Science Foundation under grant number 1942318 (to J.D.S.). Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

  • Andrews, T. C., Auerbach, A. J. J., & Grant, E. F. ( 2019 ). Exploring the relationship between teacher knowledge and active-learning implementation in large college biology courses . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 18 (4), ar48. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Baeten, M., Kyndt, E., Struyven, K., & Dochy, F. ( 2010 ). Using student-centred learning environments to stimulate deep approaches to learning: Factors encouraging or discouraging their effectiveness . Educational Research Review , 5 (3), 243–260. Google Scholar
  • Carr, M., & Taasoobshirazi, G. ( 2008 ). Metacognition in the gifted: Connections to expertise . In Shaughnessy, M. F.Veenman, M.Kleyn-Kennedy, C. (Eds.), Meta-cognition: A recent review of research, theory and perspectives (pp. 109–125). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Sciences Publishers. Google Scholar
  • Choi, M. M., & Kuo, S. W. ( 2009 ). Social metacognition in groups: Benefits, difficulties, learning and teaching . In Larson, C. B. (Ed.), Metacognition: New research developments (pp. 117–136). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. Google Scholar
  • Cross, D. R., & Paris, S. G. ( 1988 ). Developmental and instructional analyses of children’s metacognition and reading comprehension . Journal of Educational Psychology , 80 (2), 131–142. Google Scholar
  • de Carvalho Filho, M. K. ( 2009 ). Confidence judgments in real classroom settings: Monitoring performance in different types of tests . International Journal of Psychology , 44 (2), 93–108. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. ( 2013 ). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology . Psychological Science in the Public Interest , 14 (1), 4–58. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Dye, K. M., & Stanton, J. D. ( 2017 ). Metacognition in upper-division biology students: Awareness does not always lead to control . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 16 (2), ar31. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Flavell, J. H. ( 1979 ). Meta-cognition and cognitive monitoring—New area of cognitive-developmental inquiry . American Psychologist , 34 (10), 906–911. Google Scholar
  • Goos, M., Galbraith, P., & Renshaw, P. ( 2002 ). Socially mediated metacognition: Creating collaborative zones of proximal development in small group problem solving . Educational Studies in Mathematics , 49 (2), 193–223. Google Scholar
  • Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. ( 2012 ). Study strategies of college students: Are self-testing and scheduling related to achievement? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 19 (1), 126–134. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Hattie, J. A., & Donoghue, G. M. ( 2016 ). Learning strategies: A synthesis and conceptual model . Nature Partner Journal Science of Learning , 1 (1), 1–13. Google Scholar
  • Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. , III . ( 2009 ). Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Memory , 17 (4), 471–479. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. ( 2005 ). Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Cognition , 31 (2), 187–194. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. ( 1996 ). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy . Psychological Review , 103 (3), 490–517. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. ( 2008 ). Learning concepts and categories is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science , 19 (6), 585–592. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Kuhbandner, C., & Emmerdinger, K. J. ( 2019 ). Do students really prefer repeated rereading over testing when studying textbooks? A reexamination . Memory , 27 (7), 952–961. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • McGuire, S. Y. ( 2006 ). The impact of supplemental instruction on teaching students how to learn . New Directions for Teaching and Learning , 2006 (106), 3–10. Google Scholar
  • Miller, M., & Hadwin, A. F. ( 2015 ). Scripting and awareness tools for regulating collaborative learning: Changing the landscape of support in CSCL . Computers in Human Behavior , 52 , 573–588. Google Scholar
  • Nelson, T., & Narens, L. ( 1990 ). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings . Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 26 , 125–141. Google Scholar
  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. ( 2007 ). Improving students’ self-evaluation of learning for key concepts in textbook materials . European Journal of Cognitive Psychology , 19 (4–5), 559–579. Google Scholar
  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. ( 2006 ). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention . Psychological Science , 17 (3), 249–255. Medline ,  Google Scholar
  • Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C.-N. ( 2020 ). A randomized controlled trial of interleaved mathematics practice . Journal of Educational Psychology , 112 (1), 40. Google Scholar
  • Sabel, J. L., Dauer, J. T., & Forbes, C. T. ( 2017 ). Introductory biology students’ use of enhanced answer keys and reflection questions to engage in metacognition and enhance understanding . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 16 (3), ar40. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. ( 1995 ). Metacognitive theories . Educational Psychology Review , 7 (4), 351–371. Google Scholar
  • Scouller, K. ( 1998 ). The influence of assessment method on students’ learning approaches: Multiple choice question examination versus assignment essay . Higher Education , 35 (4), 453–472. Google Scholar
  • Sebesta, A. J., & Bray Speth, E. ( 2017 ). How should I study for the exam? Self-regulated learning strategies and achievement in introductory biology . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 16 (2), ar30. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Stanton, J. D., Dye, K. M., & Johnson, M. S. ( 2019 ). Knowledge of learning makes a difference: A comparison of metacognition in introductory and senior-level biology students . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 18 (2), ar24. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Stanton, J. D., Neider, X. N., Gallegos, I. J., & Clark, N. C. ( 2015 ). Differences in metacognitive regulation in introductory biology students: When prompts are not enough . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 14 (2), ar15. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Stylianou-Georgiou, A., & Papanastasiou, E. C. ( 2017 ). Answer changing in testing situations: The role of metacognition in deciding which answers to review . Educational Research and Evaluation , 23 (3–4), 102–118. Google Scholar
  • Tanner, K. D. ( 2012 ). Promoting student metacognition . CBE—Life Sciences Education , 11 (2), 113–120. Link ,  Google Scholar
  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M., & Therriault, D. ( 2003 ). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts . Journal of Educational Psychology , 95 (1), 66–73. Google Scholar
  • Van De Bogart, K. L., Dounas-Frazer, D. R., Lewandowski, H., & Stetzer, M. R. ( 2017 ). Investigating the role of socially mediated metacognition during collaborative troubleshooting of electric circuits . Physical Review Physics Education Research , 13 (2), 020116. Google Scholar
  • Veenman, M. V. J., van Hout-Wolters, B. H., & Afflerbach, P. ( 2006 ). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations . Metacognition and Learning , 1 (1), 3–14. Google Scholar
  • Wang, M. C., Haertel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. ( 1990 ). What influences learning? A content analysis of review literature . Journal of Educational Research , 84 (1), 30–43. Google Scholar
  • Stephanie M. Halmo ,
  • Kira A. Yamini , and
  • Elisabeth Schussler, Monitoring Editor
  • Stephanie M. Gardner ,
  • Aakanksha Angra , and
  • Joseph A. Harsh
  • Kristy J Wilson, Monitoring Editor
  • Diane K. Angell ,
  • Sharon Lane-Getaz ,
  • Taylor Okonek , and
  • Stephanie Smith
  • Jennifer Knight, Monitoring Editor
  • Jennifer K. Knight ,
  • Daniel C. Weaver ,
  • Melanie E. Peffer , and
  • Zachary S. Hazlett
  • Tessa C. Andrews, Monitoring Editor
  • Emily K. Bremers ,
  • Sammantha Fuller , and
  • Stephanie Gardner, Monitoring Editor
  • Undergraduates' Experiences with Online and in-Person Courses Provide Opportunities for Improving Student-Centered Biology Laboratory Instruction Journal of Microbiology & Biology Education, Vol. 23, No. 1
  • Student attitudes in an innovative active learning approach in calculus 4 February 2022 | International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, Vol. 5
  • Lauren Hensley ,
  • Amy Kulesza ,
  • Joshua Peri ,
  • Anna C. Brady ,
  • Christopher A. Wolters ,
  • David Sovic , and
  • Caroline Breitenberger
  • Joel K. Abraham, Monitoring Editor
  • Persistent Miscalibration for Low and High Achievers despite Practice Test Feedback in an Introductory Biology Course Journal of Microbiology & Biology Education, Vol. 22, No. 2

metacognitive knowledge essay

© 2021 J. D. Stanton et al. CBE—Life Sciences Education © 2021 The American Society for Cell Biology. This article is distributed by The American Society for Cell Biology under license from the author(s). It is available to the public under an Attribution–Noncommercial–Share Alike 3.0 Unported Creative Commons License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/3.0).

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

A questionnaire-based validation of metacognitive strategies in writing and their predictive effects on the writing performance of English as foreign language student writers

Ruru Zhang, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5654-2402

Yanling Xiao, https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0025-2024

Associated Data

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/ Supplementary material , further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Introduction

This study—drawing upon data from a questionnaire—examined 503 Chinese university students’ metacognitive strategies in writing (MSW). The focus was on Chinese student writers who are learning English as a foreign language (EFL).

The examination was conducted through a survey on MSW and a writing test administered at the end of the semester. We employed exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for data analysis. Multiple regression analysis was also adopted for understanding the predictive effects of strategies on writing performance.

The findings provided validity to MSW, including person, task, strategies, planning, monitoring, and evaluating. The different components of MSW were reported to significantly affect the participants’ writing performance. The findings highlight that EFL student writers were aware of metacognitive writing strategies. The MSW survey could be used to assess EFL students’ metacognitive writing strategies and develop curricula in writing strategy training.

Writing instruction can direct learners’ ability to acquire metacognitive writing strategies, particularly those of planning, monitoring, and evaluating, to build their awareness as agents in EFL writing. Relevant pedagogical implications are discussed.

Metacognitive strategies are essential to the process of learning to write when learning English as a foreign language (EFL; Nguyen and Gu, 2013 ; Teng, 2016 , 2019 ; Teng and Yue,2022 ). However, in the Chinese EFL context, for which English writing instruction typically emphasizes grammatical correctness rather than idea development, learners may find it difficult to build an awareness of using metacognitive writing strategies ( Ruan, 2014 ). Through a mixed-methods study, Amani (2014) found that explicit metacognitive strategy instruction had a positive impact on the writing competence of L2 writing students. However, in terms of EFL writing, university EFL students may find it challenging because of their lack of awareness of metacognitive writing strategies ( Teng, 2019 ). In addition, EFL learners in the Chinese context receive limited English language input, making it more challenging to learn to write. Student writers are expected to have repertoires of strategies when learning to write ( Raimes, 1987 ). In particular, they need to build an advanced level of “self-initiated thoughts, feelings, and actions” for them to “attain various literary goals” ( Zimmerman and Risemberg, 1997 , p.76). Hence, metacognitive writing strategies are essential to possible improvements in EFL writing.

Nevertheless, even though students are taught how to plan, monitor, and evaluate their own writing, students may know little about themselves as writers ( Leung and Hicks, 2014 ). They may also not recognize their own writing strengths or weaknesses, tending to overemphasize the latter and overlook any progress they have made or can make in their writing ( Teng, 2016 ). Wenden (1998) argued that metacognitive knowledge is a prerequisite for self-regulation, and metacognitive knowledge is essential to learner autonomy because it “informs planning decisions taken at the outset of learning and the monitoring processes that regulate the completion of a learning task and decisions to remediate; it also provides the criteria for evaluation made once a learning task is completed” (p. 528). Teng and Zhang (2021) argued that there is a dynamic and longitudinal relationship between metacognitive knowledge and reading and writing in a foreign language context. However, teachers may not recognize the importance of metacognitive knowledge in Chinese EFL writing contexts, wherein teaching academic writing is product oriented ( Teng and Zhang, 2016 ). The student writers were passive and found it difficult to keep positive beliefs in writing ( Bruning and Horn, 2000 ). This may be related to learners’ lack of awareness of self-regulation in writing. They may exert more effort learning vocabulary knowledge and grammar for writing, rather than being an agent for writing ( Graham and Harris, 2000 ). Student writers need self-awareness, motivation, and positive behavioral skills for writing ( Zimmerman, 2002 , p.65–66). Metacognitive writing strategies are thus essential to EFL students’ writing performance.

Self-regulation principles, measurements, and practices have a solid ground for enriching second and foreign language learning and teaching ( Teng and Zhang, 2022 ). Through a socio-cognitive approach to writing, Nishino and Atkinson (2015) argued that writing is primarily a cognitive activity and that cognition plays a vital role in writing and its development. To help students become competent English writers and autonomous learners, instructors need to support their development of metacognitive strategies. However, scarce attention was paid to writing strategies from the perspective of metacognition, particularly for low-achieving students in the EFL context. The present study examined Chinese university EFL students’ metacognitive strategies in EFL writing. We aim for the following purposes: (a) to assess the reliability of a new scale, which we named it as metacognitive strategies in writing (MSW) and (b) to explore how different components of MSW predict EFL students’ writing performance. The findings are insightful in helping researchers and classroom practitioners to diagnose the needs of metacognitive strategies in writing and develop guidelines for instructing writing courses for university EFL students. The findings shed lights on how to teach EFL writing and deliver more effective program for writing teacher preparation.

Literature review

Language learning strategies.

Oxford (1990) classified a list of language learning strategies based on cognitive learning theory. These strategies include memory, cognitive, compensatory, affective, social, and metacognitive strategies. Past studies have documented differences in strategy use between more and less successful learners. For example, successful learners use these strategies in larger numbers and at higher frequencies ( Magogwe and Oliver, 2007 ). Most importantly, cognitive and metacognitive strategies are associated with a higher level of language proficiency ( Peacock and Ho, 2003 ). However, contradictory findings were also reported, showing that less successful learners used more strategies than more successful learners did because the former automatized their language learning process ( Oxford and Cohen, 1992 ). Another point worth noting is that unsuccessful learners may adopt a large number of strategies frequently, but it does not necessarily mean that they are able to identify appropriate strategy use. In fact, it was reported that successful learners were able to identify appropriate strategies depending on the task requirements, but unsuccessful learners failed to choose the most appropriate and efficient strategies during the task ( Chamot and El-Dinary, 1999 ).

Although ample research has been reported relating to learners’ proficiency level and strategy use, learner variables, such as cultural background and national origin, could have a strong influence on learners’ strategy use ( Oxford and Nyikos, 1989 ). Therefore, their findings might not be generalizable to learners with completely different cultural backgrounds. In light of this, Lai (2009) conducted a questionnaire survey that investigated the relationships between the language learning strategies used by 418 EFL learners in Taiwan based on learners’ language proficiency and their use of strategies. While the more proficient learners used metacognitive strategies and cognitive strategies most frequently and memory strategies least frequently, the less proficient learners preferred social and memory strategies to cognitive and metacognitive strategies. This finding partially echoes Wu (2008) , who reported that higher-proficiency EFL students in Taiwan used learning strategies more often than lower-proficiency EFL students did, especially the cognitive, metacognitive and social strategies.

Although research documented in the literature examines general language learning strategy use, it is possible that these summarized findings could serve as a reference for the specific examination of metacognitive strategy use during English writing.

Understanding metacognition

Metacognition is multidimensional and domain-general. When we talk about metacognition, we may need to mention the theory of mind ( Flavell, 1979 ). Such theory is the foundation of understanding metacognition. Generally, metacognition is related to self-regulatory capacity because metacognition provides individuals with domain knowledge and regulatory skills that are essential to become an agentive learner in relevant domains ( Schraw, 2001 , p. 7). Metacognition refers to how learners build an awareness of their own thinking processes and executive processes ( Flavell, 1979 ). Metacognition is essential to helping learners regulate their cognitive processes, and finally, becoming an independent thinker and learner. Zhang and Zhang (2019) applied metacognition in second and foreign language learning, and posited that EFL learners need to plan, monitor, and evaluate their cognitive processes for better language learning performance.

Metacognition includes metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive regulation. Flavell (1985) suggested that person, task, and strategy knowledge are three key elements of metacognitive knowledge. Wenden (1998) explained the three elements. For example, person knowledge is the knowledge for the learners to control their cognitive processes. Task knowledge is the knowledge that can be helpful for the learners to understand the purpose, nature, and demands of different task conditions. Strategy knowledge is the knowledge of different important strategies that are helpful for realizing the pre-determined goals. Metacognitive regulation entails three skills: planning, monitoring, and evaluating ( Schraw, 1998 ). Planning refers to the ability to appropriately select the strategies and adequately allocate the resources for completing tasks. Monitoring refers to learners’ capacity to observe their task performance. Evaluating means learners’ capacity to reflect on their learning outcome and the use of different strategies for self-regulation.

Teng et al. (2022) summarized the procedures of understanding metacognition. First, monitoring function and control of cognition are two important functions of metacognition. In order to realize the functions, individuals need to process three major stages, i.e., acquisition, retention, and retrieval. Second, learners need metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive experiences to process the monitoring function. In contrast, they need metacognitive strategies or metacognitive skills to fulfill the needs of control of cognition. Third, metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive experiences, and metacognitive skills are interconnected with each other. Metacognitive knowledge includes person, task, and strategies. Metacognitive experiences include feelings and judgments. Metacognitive skills are important for their metacognitive regulation, which needs learners to plan, monitor, and evaluate their learning process. Finally, reflection is the outcome of the interconnected process of planning, monitoring, and evaluating ( Figure 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-1071907-g001.jpg

The multifaceted elements of metacognition ( Teng et al., 2022 , p. 171).

Metacognitive strategies in EFL writing

Macaro (2010) maintains that strategic behavior plays a vital role in second language learning success and proposes that strategic behavior should be essential to linguistic knowledge resources. Dornyei (2010) emphasizes that students need a repertoire of appropriate task-related plans, scripts, and self-regulatory strategies that are activated by their ideal L2 selves; that is, learners’ aptitude, motivation, goals, and self-regulatory strategies all interact and affect one another in the SLA process. Writing strategies include rhetorical strategies, metacognitive strategies, cognitive strategies, and social/affective strategies ( Wenden, 1991 ; Riazi, 1997 ). Writers explore rhetorical strategies to organize and present their ideas based on the writing conventions of the target language. Metacognitive strategies are used to monitor the writing process consciously and evaluate the effectiveness of writing actions. Cognitive strategies are used to implement actual writing actions. Social/affective strategies are employed to interact with others and to regulate emotions, motivation, and attitudes in writing.

Wenden (1991) classifies writing strategies based on metacognitive and cognitive frameworks. She distinguishes general executive metacognitive strategies of planning, self-monitoring, and self-evaluating from more specific cognitive strategies, such as clarification, retrieval, resourcing, avoidance, and verification. Each of these metacognitive strategies is discussed below.

Planning for writing involves thinking and self-questioning strategies such as identifying one’s purpose, activating background knowledge, and organizing ideas. Planning is not limited to a specific stage of writing but rather appears recursively throughout the writing process. Flower and Hayes (1981) identified three different types of planning strategies based on the focus of the goal: (1) generating ideas; (2) setting procedural goals; and (3) organizing. Generating ideas includes retrieving information from long-term memory, revising old ideas to incorporate new information, drawing inferences, making connections, and looking for examples, contradictions, and objections. Setting procedural goals includes content goals (e.g., plans for content, text structure and audience, and criteria for evaluation) and process goals (how to proceed, generated by the writer, done at any time during the composing process, followed or preceded by generating ideas, revising strategies, etc.). The third strategy (organizing) includes selecting the most useful materials produced during the generating process and organizing them in the writing plan. Organizing strategies include grouping and sequencing ideas, deciding on the presentation of the text, planning the introduction and conclusions, and structuring the text based on a particular genre. Furthermore, in using these strategies, it is essential to consider the audience, topic, and rhetorical knowledge. Planning in EFL writing determines how writers write in subsequent stages. It engages them in metacognitive activities that allow them to consider the purpose and goals for writing, identify their audience, decide upon voice, and generate a framework for their essays.

Monitoring involves conscious control and regulation of the writing process. Hayes and Flower (1980) include self-monitoring in their model of the cognitive processes of writing, noting that the ability to self-monitor the composing process is an important part of writing strategies. Charles (1990) claims that self-monitoring makes it easier for L2 students to avoid uncertainty about any part of their text, to find direct answers to their queries and to encourage them “to look critically and analytically at their writing and to place themselves in the position of readers” (p. 289). The more important functions of self-monitoring are controlling, directing, and sequencing the composing processes and one’s progress in the task. Monitoring allows the writer to decide whether something needs to be retrieved, whether new ideas need to be further generated, or whether a given subprocess has ended. Monitoring allows L2 writers to evaluate the effectiveness of writing strategies and how and when to check the outcomes of problem-solving processes and strategically regulate the processes according to cognitive goals ( Mayer, 1999 ).

Self-evaluating—experiencing the quality of one’s writing in relation to one’s goals—is crucial for developing an individual’s perception of writing. In self-evaluation, students can recognize weaknesses, identify needs, and make changes ( Zimmerman, 2002 ). In cognitive research, evaluation has been characterized as a strategy for considering the outcome of the undertaken task, an essential metacognitive strategy that successful learners need to execute and control.

Empirical studies on the use of metacognitive writing strategies

Various studies have been conducted on EFL students’ use of metacognitive writing strategies. Employing think-aloud protocols and immediate retrospective interviews, Chien (2012) investigated the differences in writing strategies and English writing achievements of 20 low-achieving and 20 high-achieving student writers in Taiwan. Chien found that high-achieving student writers were more aware of and focused more on, formulating their position statements when planning, generating, revising, and editing their essays and focused more on correcting grammatical and spelling errors. Teng and Zhang (2016) validated questionnaire-based self-regulated strategies in EFL writing and highlighted planning, monitoring, and evaluating in EFL writing. Teng and Huang (2019) also suggested that learners’ self-regulated strategies in writing, as well as their English proficiency and language learning experiences, and significantly influenced their EFL writing. In a recent publication ( Teng et al., 2022 ), two experimental studies were reported. Study 1 adopted a factorial design using exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis to validate a self-regulatory writing strategy questionnaire. Study 2 assessed the predictive effects of the different components of the scale on students’ writing performance. The results supported the construct validity for the six strategy factors, i.e., writing planning, goal-oriented monitoring, goal-oriented evaluation, emotional control, memorization, and metacognitive judgment. The factors also predicted writing performance. Zhang and Qin (2018) also validated the newly developed scale on metacognitive strategies in a multimedia writing context. The results provided evidence for the validation of planning, monitoring, and evaluating strategies. In an early empirical study on the importance of planning in EFL writing, Graham et al. (1995) examined differences between expert and less-skilled L2 writers. They found that expert L2 writers spent considerable time planning and appeared to have higher-level plans and self-conscious control of their planning. In contrast, less-skilled EFL writers were less likely to use knowledge of textual structure in planning, to use heuristic strategies in searching their memory for content, or to establish goals to direct the writing process and were more likely to engage in “knowledge telling” (i.e., writing everything they knew about a topic and stopping when they felt that they had written down everything they knew). Less-skilled writers did not write with goals or plans in mind; rather, they tended to generate ideas through free writing and usually did not organize those ideas. As shown in a longitudinal study ( Teng and Zhang, 2021 ), learners’ L2 writing development was dependent on their initial level of metacognitive knowledge. This is evidence for the strong correlation between metacognitive knowledge and writing.

Nguyen and Gu (2013) explored the impact of strategy-based instruction on promoting learner autonomy (operationally defined as learner self-initiation and learner self-regulation) of students at a Vietnamese university; 37 students were in an experimental group, and 54 students were in two control groups. After an 8-week metacognition training intervention, students in the experimental group were found to have improved their planning, monitoring, and evaluating of a writing task more than those in the two control groups. The findings suggest that strategy-based instruction on task-specific metacognitive self-regulation improves learner autonomy and writing performance. Teng (2020) also incorporated training of metacognitive strategies for EFL learners. There were two groups of learners, i.e., those with group feedback guidance and those with self-explanation guidance. The results supported the positive effects of group metacognitive support on EFL students’ writing. EFL students need to build a certain level of metacognitive awareness to manage themselves as writers.

Bai et al. (2014) conducted a questionnaire survey to explore the relationship between 1,618 Singapore primary school pupils’ reported use of strategies in learning to write and the correlation with their English language proficiency. They found that participants used a wide range of writing strategies at medium frequency. They also reported a significant correlation between the participants’ English language proficiency and the use of writing strategies such as planning, text-generating, revising, monitoring and evaluating, and resourcing. Similar results were also found in Bai and Guo (2021) , wherein high achievers reported higher levels of motivation (i.e., growth mindset, self-efficacy, and interest) and self-regulated learning strategy use than the average achievers, and average achievers reported more strategy use than the low achievers, Ma and Teng (2021) collected qualitative data from two undergraduate university students learning English as L2 in Hong Kong to explore their use of writing strategies. They reported that both students realized the importance of self-evaluation and revision. It seems that the students perceived affordances in the kind of writing that enabled them to play an active role in seeking, interpreting, and using teacher feedback to perform the evaluation and modification of their own work. However, variations in engagement in the process of learning to write and their metacognitive knowledge development were also detected. For example, students’ varying degrees of engagement may result in various degrees of developing metacognitive awareness. Teng et al. (2022) validated a new instrument, i.e., the Metacognitive Academic Writing Strategies Questionnaire (MAWSQ). Analyses were conducted through a series of Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA). Results supported two hypothesized models, i.e., an eight-factor correlated model and a one-factor second-order model. Model comparisons supported the role of metacognition as a higher-order construct. Metacognition also explains the eight metacognitive strategies, including declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge, conditional knowledge, planning, monitoring, evaluating, information management, and debugging strategies. Those strategies also significantly influenced EFL writing performance.

Overall, the studies on metacognition development reviewed in this section highlight the importance of the high-level cognitive processes involved in composing, the development of the autonomous and self-regulated use of effective writing strategies, and the formation of positive attitudes about writing. Metacognitively oriented learners are aware of both their own learner characteristics and the writing task and are able to select, employ, monitor, and evaluate their use of metacognitive strategies.

The present study

Metacognition functions as an important predictor in EFL writing performance. We aim for two purposes in the present study. First, we attempted to validate a questionnaire on metacognitive strategies in writing. Second, we assessed the predictive effects of different metacognitive strategies in the outcome EFL writing. The present study sheds light on learners’ awareness and use of metacognitive writing strategies. The present study includes two questions:

  • What is the evidence to support the validity and reliability of metacognitive strategies in writing?
  • What is the evidence for the predictive effects of metacognitive strategies on EFL writing proficiency?

Materials and methods

Participants.

The present study included 503 participants. They were undergraduate students at a university in China. They were first-year students with Chinese as their first language and English as a foreign language. They had received at least 6 years of formal English instruction. Writing is a subject to be taught in college English and a compulsory course for all the participants. We selected the participants because they were all enrolled in a university English course. The first author was teaching the participants, and the sample of participants was a convenient sample. Among the 503 students, 351 were men and 152 were women. An unequal gender balance may be because most of the students were from science and engineering majors. Originally, there were 700 students who responded to the questionnaire. We finally selected data from 503 students for data analysis. Some participants’ data were excluded because of missing values or because some were unable to take the writing test. They attended the study voluntarily by signing the consent form.

Questionnaire development

The questionnaire, which was named Metacognitive Strategies in Writing (MSW), was developed through item generation, reference consultation, initial piloting, psychometric evaluation, and exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in a pilot study. We first invited 10 students to reflect on their writing practices and strategies. The students were mainly interviewed about the strategies they adopted for writing. We generated approximately 50 items based on analyzing the transcriptions of learners’ interviews. In the next stage, we consulted relevant literature on metacognition, self-regulation, and language learning strategies ( Schraw and Dennison, 1994 ; Oxford, 2013 ; Teng et al., 2022 ). We selected the items that fit with metacognition theories. In the third stage, we invited the 10 students to check the items. In the fourth stage, which was psychometric evaluation, we invited two researchers in L2 writing to assess the items. Based on the comments, we finally removed 10 items. In the final stage, we ran an EFA with a sample of 360 students with similar backgrounds. We deleted 10 items with unsatisfactory factor loading values. The final questionnaire includes 30 items, which are in the Appendix .

This questionnaire was a novel one as it was based on metacognition theory, through which the focus was on understanding metacognitive knowledge and regulation in learning to write. We adopted a seven-point Likert scale (i.e., from 1, Strongly disagree to 7, Strongly agree). MSW focuses on metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive regulation. Metacognitive knowledge includes three factors, i.e., person, task, and strategies. Metacognitive regulation includes three factors: planning, monitoring, and evaluating. Cronbach’s alpha, which ranged from 0.81 to 0.90 for the six factors, ensured the internal consistency of responses to the items. The questionnaires were administered to the participants in Chinese. The author translated into Chinese while a research assistant was invited to check the translated items through back translation.

Writing test

A writing test from IELTS (writing task 2) was adopted to measure learners’ writing proficiency. Students were required to write at least 250 words within 1 h. Students were asked to respond to the topic provided by giving and justifying an opinion, discussing the topic, summarizing details, outlining problems, identifying possible solutions and supporting what they wrote with reasons, arguments and relevant examples. The topic proposed the possible influence of social media sites on personal relationships.

The marking scheme was consistent with the writing rubrics in IELTS. However, we adjusted it to fit with our school assessment needs. Each learner was awarded with six marks for task response, coherence and cohesion, lexical resource, and grammatical range and accuracy. The maximum possible score was 24 points. A total of 40 English teachers were paid to rate the writing. The teachers did not know the participants’ identities. They also joined a training session on the marking scheme. Disagreements on marking were subject to further discussion. The Cronbach’s alpha for the test was.85, indicating acceptable reliability.

We invited 20 EFL teachers to help us distribute a QR code to the students through WeChat group. The students spent an average of 6 min completing the questionnaire. The writing test was administered as an exercise for all students during class. They needed to complete it within 1 h. The format for the writing test was a paper-and-pencil format. All participants received the same format for the questionnaire and the writing test.

Data analysis

The final dataset was run through a series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs). STATA was used for data analysis. CFA is used to test a theoretical model by confirming factors, correlations, covariance patterns, and residual or error values within a data matrix ( Byrne, 2016 ). We used the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method. The model fit was evaluated through the following statistics: a chi-square statistic, the degrees of freedom (df), p value, the ratio of chi-square χ 2 divided by the df, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; DiStefano and Hess, 2005 ). The following criteria are a relatively good fit between the hypothesized model and the observed data: the value of RMSEA should be close to 0.06, the value of SRMR should be close to 0.08, and the values for CFI and TLI should be close to 0.95 ( Hu and Bentler, 1999 ). Finally, multiple regression analysis was adopted to evaluate the predictive effects of MSW on students’ writing proficiency.

Descriptive statistics

The kurtosis and skewness values for the metacognitive strategies in writing, as well as the mean and standard deviation, are shown in Table 1 . The means of the six factors ranged from 3.346 to 4.079, with the two factors, monitoring and evaluating, greater than 4. There were no noticeable variations based on the standard deviation values.

Means, standard deviations. and normality test.

Exploratory factor analysis in the pilot study

Exploratory factor analysis was conducted on a sample of 360 learners from similar background in the pilot study. We examined the adequacy of the sample. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin value was 0.914, which appropriate for EFA ( Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001 ). Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, p < 0 .001; thus, the matrix was adequate for factor analysis. We adopted principal component analysis as a factor extraction method. We finally extracted six factors that explained 57.411% of the variance ( Table 2 ). The scree plot showed a considerable drop after the sixth factor, for which we excluded other possible factors. Based on key theories in metacognition, we named the six factors as following: person, task, strategies, planning, monitoring, and evaluating.

Extraction results for the six factors.

The six factors’ eigenvalues exceeded 1. The next step was to examine the factor loadings. We deleted 10 items with factor loadings lower than 0.4. The final version included 30 items across six factors ( Table 3 ). Items’ factor loadings ranged from 0.534 to 0.772, while communality ranged from 0.531 to 0.754. The items hence fit their respective factors well.

Results on factor loadings and the communality.

Construct validity of metacognitive strategies in writing through CFA

The data fitness metrics for metacognitive strategies in writing are displayed in Table 4 . Table 4 shows that the RMSEA was 0.073, less than 0.08, indicating a good fit; CFI, TLI, CNFI, IFI, and GFI all exceeded 0.9, which was ideal for adaptability. Although the χ 2 /df was 7.916, larger than 3, the scale on metacognitive strategies in writing still showed reliability when taken as a whole.

Model fit indices for metacognitive writing strategies.

According to Figure 2 and Table 5 , the factor loadings for Person, Task, Strategy, Planning and Evaluating were all greater than 0.5, while Monitoring was 0.41. Additionally, the average variance extracted (AVE) for each variable was 0.47, and the model’s convergent validity was good, as evidenced by the composite reliability (CR) being 0.84, indicating that the model had satisfactory convergent validity.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-1071907-g002.jpg

A first-order model of metacognitive strategies in writing. Prs, Person; Tsk, Task; Str, Strategy; Pln, Planning; Mnt, Monitoring; and Evl, Evaluating.

Convergent validity of the model.

Predictive effect of metacognitive strategies in writing on EFL writing

Figure 3 presents the correlations between metacognitive strategies in writing and L2 learners’ writing proficiency in English. The findings indicated that each of the six metacognitive strategies was significantly correlated with learners’ English writing performance. Writing performance (WP) was correlated with Person ( r  = 0.264), Task ( r  = 0.500), Planning ( r  = 0.584), and Monitoring ( r  = 0.408). Strategy ( r  = 0.470) and Evaluating ( r  = 0.470) were significantly correlated with WP.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-1071907-g003.jpg

Spearman correlation for metacognitive writing strategies and L2 learners’ proficiency in English. Persontotal, Person; Tasktotal, Task; Strategytotal, Strategy; Planningtotal, Planning; Monitoringtotal, Monitoring; and Evaluatingtotal, Evaluating.

Moreover, we adopted a structural equation model to investigate the degree to which metacognitive strategies in writing predicted learners’ L2 writing proficiency. Table 6 presents the model fitness indices. For our model, seven indices (i.e., χ 2 /df, RMSEA, CFI, TLI, NFI, WIFI, and GFI) indicated acceptable model fit ( Table 6 ). Figure 4 shows a structural equation model of the relationship between metacognitive strategies in writing and writing proficiency. The six variables on the left side of the model represent the six factors of metacognitive strategies in writing. The only rectangular variable on the right side of the model was EFL learners’ writing proficiency. The findings demonstrated that metacognitive strategies in writing had a predictive power of 0.65 for L2 learners’ writing proficiency, indicating that it could account for 65% of the variances in writing performance.

Model fit indices for metacognitive writing strategies on writing performance.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-1071907-g004.jpg

The structural equation model of metacognitive strategies in writing proficiency.

Regression analysis was employed in the study to show the extent to which each factor impacts writing performance. The results presented in Table 7 demonstrate that all factors significantly predicted writing competence ( p  < 0.001), with the exception of Strategy ( p  = 0.344). Planning had the greatest effect on writing abilities, and Task had the least effect. Notably, monitoring and evaluating also had a great effect on EFL learners’ writing proficiency. According to the findings, there was no multicollinearity among the strategies, as indicated by the variance inflation factor (VIF), which was less than 3. In addition, the residuals adhered to a normal distribution, as shown in Figure 5 . This offered a trustworthy foundation for the regression analysis results.

Linear regression results.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-13-1071907-g005.jpg

Normal P–P plot of regression standardized residual.

Discussion and conclusion

Overall, the present study aims to answer two research questions. The first research question entails the validation of a newly developed scale, which we named Metacognitive Strategies in Writing (MSW). The scale was developed based on metacognition theory. The findings supported the factorial structure of the scale. The second research question aims to answer the predictive effects of different factors of MSW in writing performance. Overall, the findings provided evidence for the factorial structure of MSW. The findings also suggested the predictive effects of different factors on writing performance.

Validation of MSW

First, MSW is with satisfactory psychometric properties. The six factors were reliable in terms of conceptual and empirical evidence. The six factors were distinct but correlated with each other. Consistent with previous studies ( Teng et al., 2022 ), metacognition is an important construct that can explain the significant correlations of different lower-order metacognitive dimensions in writing. In line with Schraw and Moshman (1995) , metacognition is a domain that can explain self-regulatory capacity. The present study thus provides insights into metacognition theory, which can entail person, task, strategies, planning, monitoring, and evaluating ( Schraw and Dennison, 1994 ). These strategies are interconnected and reflect the metacognitive process in writing. To build metacognitive awareness, learners need to be engaged in self-reflection and controlling of cognition ( Paris and Winograd, 1990 ). In terms of writing, student writers need to assess their knowledge states and executive abilities to orchestrate different dimensions of metacognitive awareness. Overall, the sum of the six strategies in writing indicates EFL student writers’ overall level of metacognitive awareness in writing.

The six factors were interpreted through metacognitive knowledge and regulation. The two paradigms were also conceptualized in early studies ( Flavell, 1979 ; Schraw, 1998 ; Wenden, 1998 ). In the present study, the two paradigms can represent key elements of metacognition. Person, task, and strategies represent learners’ beliefs and knowledge about themselves. Planning, monitoring, and evaluating reflect the process of cultivating one’ self-regulatory capacity for learning to write ( Teng and Zhang, 2016 ; Teng et al., 2022 ). The findings showed a positive and significant relationship between metacognitive knowledge and regulation ( Pugalee, 2001 ; Teng, 2016 ). We may need to reconsider the strong connection between metacognitive knowledge and regulation. The positive correlation may reflect the need of both knowledge and regulation in learning to write. For example, EFL students may need cognitive, metacognitive, and regulatory skills and strategies for writing ( Teng, 2020 ). The importance of metacognitive knowledge and regulation may reflect the argument by Wolters (1999) that learners’ engagement, effort, and achievement are influenced by their metacognitive knowledge and regulation. Hence, metacognition is essential to the development of self-regulated capacity ( Efklides, 2008 ), build identity as a student writer ( Zimmerman and Risemberg, 1997 , p.76), and develop self-awareness in processing their second and foreign language learning ( Zhang and Zhang, 2019 ).

Overall, the MSW data suggest that the student writers adopted metacognitive knowledge, i.e., person, task, and strategies, to understand their strengths and weakness in writing, demands in writing, and solutions for solving problems in writing. The data also suggest that the planning strategy should be used. In the planning stage, the student writers directed their attention to fulfilling the goal of the task, planning thoroughly, evaluating the relevance and effectiveness of ideas, and eliminating inappropriate examples. Data regarding the second subscale (monitoring) reflected that students tended to use some metacognitive monitoring strategies. During the monitoring stage, the student writers focused on the overall essay development, concentrating on expanding and developing their initial ideas, evaluating their essay for clear development and focus/unity, and ignoring interruptions posed by language constraints, such as grammar and vocabulary. For the third subscale (self-evaluating), student writers tended to use certain metacognitive strategies. Student writers prioritized their attention to evaluating the unity and effectiveness of their writing before editing local errors, such as grammar, vocabulary, mechanics, and sentence variation.

Predictive effects of metacognitive strategies in writing

The findings suggest the predictive effects of metacognitive strategies in writing. The results confirmed that the metacognitive strategies significantly predicted learners’ writing performance, which was consistent with previous studies ( Teng and Huang, 2019 ; Teng et al., 2022 ). One reason is that student writers’ meager metacognitive knowledge base could result in unsatisfactory cognitive monitoring of production and progress toward the writing task goal, which, in turn, may also affect their writing performance ( Teng et al., 2022 ). For example, lower-level writers tended to be bound to the local areas of writing, focusing on language correctness, while higher-level writers tended to focus on developing ideas and revising at the discourse level, saving editing until later ( Teng and Huang, 2019 ). As supported in previous studies ( Chien, 2012 ; Bai et al., 2014 ), higher level student writers were more aware of metacognitive strategies and used them more frequently in writing.

The argument revealed, at least for this particular sample and the chosen test, a strong and significant link between the writing abilities of EFL students and the factors of person, task, strategy, planning, monitoring, and evaluation. The EFL learners’ writing performance variations were accounted for by the six metacognitive components. The findings complement cognitive writing model of Flower and Hayes (1981) , which recognizes the abilities in process writing such as planning, monitoring, and reviewing. Writing necessitates the adaptive use of emotional strategies, performance strategies, and cognitive strategies ( Teng et al., 2022 ). The effectiveness of the strategies highlights the personal, behavioral, and environmental impacts on the regulatory capacity in learning to write ( Zimmerman and Risemberg, 1997 ).

In our study, person and task significantly predicted writing performance with a large effect size. According to earlier research ( Brown, 1987 ; Schraw, 2001 ), learners who have declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge are more likely to become strategic learners. These results provide evidence for the idea that to master writing, EFL learners need to be able to distinguish among the various strategies, employ the appropriate strategies, and apply these strategies in their writing. The results also support earlier research that metacognitive knowledge is crucial for encouraging active involvement in applying their understanding of the writing process, recognizing the kinds of strategies useful in the growth of writing, and improving students’ writing outputs ( Ruan, 2014 ).

In terms of metacognitive regulation, planning, monitoring, and evaluating are also important for writing performance. The effect size was quite large in the current study, for which we can detect similar results in previous studies ( Teng, 2019 ; Teng et al., 2022 ). The writing abilities of students who were more self-controlled in their writing were higher in terms of goal setting, time management, and planning for writing resources ( Teng and Zhang, 2016 ). We argue that Chinese EFL students need an awareness of planning ahead and monitoring and evaluating their planning tactics to produce successful written essays. The success of EFL academic writing depends heavily on this method. Academic writing development may be seen as a complex process for student writers because it depends on how strategically they seek information and modify their planning techniques. Students who have prepared well for academic writing are typically those who have a high level of metacognitive awareness of their writing-related objectives ( Zhang and Qin, 2018 ). When composing their essays, lower-level writers often experienced difficulty in transferring ideas to paper during the planning, monitoring, and self-evaluating stages. The constraints in the lower-level writers’ knowledge system, including their limited linguistic competence (grammar and vocabulary), their confusion about their role as writers, their lack of knowledge strategies for overcoming writing difficulties, and their lack of knowledge of how and when to apply those strategies, impeded their composition of a meaningful essay. Consequently, many students tended to simultaneously engage in a few different stages of writing—planning, composing, revising, and editing—without any extra attention resources to monitor the overall unity and coherence of the essay, thus making the essay messy and confusing.

Limitations and implications

Despite the positive findings, we still need to acknowledge some limitations of this study. First, the strategies described in the questionnaire were still scarce, although we showed excellent content validity. Due to the limited amount of time the learners could invest in data collection, we did not assess metacognitive experiences, another crucial component of metacognition. Interview data with students were not conducted to yield adequate methods connected to metacognitive experiences. Second, a self-report questionnaire served as the foundation for this study. Because they are dependent on the use of self-reported information, surveys may not fully reflect learners’ actual metacognitive awareness and activities. The quantitative data in future studies should be triangulated with interview data. Third, the writing test should include additional activity categories that can gauge various writing abilities. We only used one writing performance indicator. The performance of student writers may also be impacted by individual characteristics, including their language learning experiences and English proficiency level ( Teng and Huang, 2019 ). Future studies might look at learners’ individual differences and their use of different metacognitive strategies.

However, there are also some implications based on the findings. Our findings suggest directions for pedagogy as well as future research. Considerations include issues of focus on form, development of metacognitive awareness to support metacognitive knowledge and strategies, and appreciation of the many aspects of metacognitive awareness that good L2 writing entails.

Data collected from the surveys suggest a strong connection between EFL student writers’ metacognitive knowledge and the regulation strategies they employ. Helping students become more aware of themselves as writers and the metacognitive resources upon which they can draw during the writing process may help them develop their writing competence. Language teachers and instructors should clearly instruct the importance of metacognitive strategies for EFL student writers. Related to this, metacognitive training should help students develop such awareness in learning to write. However, an important step in developing productive pedagogy for metacognitive training is assessing learners’ needs and understandings of their metacognitive strategies. The MSW might potentially contribute to EFL writing assessment in China. The MSW monitoring subscale identified the important first step in writing—planning—as a potential problem. So far as these Chinese EFL non-English major student writers were concerned, regardless of their level of English class or their majors, it seems that many of them may need to faster a metacognitive awareness. As a result, it might be helpful to provide these students with additional lessons on metacognitive strategies to address their concerns and the problems evident in their English writing. While dealing with grammatical errors is essential to writing instruction, the students should focus not only on identifying the errors and fixing them but also on finding out why they make those mistakes and how to avoid making them again. In other words, instead of correcting the errors, they should also develop their awareness of metacognitive strategies to improve their overall language competence. The instructors may also explicitly teach and demonstrate effective strategies to enhance vocabulary acquisition, such as making learners aware of lexical morphology (including word roots and suffixes), synonyms, antonyms, word categories, and similar spellings.

Clearly, it should not be assumed that learners who do not score high on norm-referenced assessments of their L2 writing need to focus exclusively on their metacognitive strategies, even though that is where they may think they need to work. Rather, these learners need to consider not only metacognitive strategies but also discourse organization and considerations of audience, voice, and genre ( Hyland, 2007 ). It is only through an approach raising their awareness of the various aspects that contribute to good writing and through work on writing and revision strategies that they will progress optimally. Additionally, to implement these recommendations for pedagogy, teachers themselves must have substantial knowledge, professional development, and practice regarding approaches to support L2 writing. In the Chinese context, knowledge must be processed and understood in light of the metacognition and experiences of students, colleagues, and the community.

Data availability statement

Ethics statement.

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Hainan University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CQ: Coordinated the study, drafted, and revised the manuscript. RZ: Data collection, drafted literature review. YX: Participated in the design of the study, revised the manuscript and performed the statistical analysis and data interpretation. All authors proofread and approved the final manuscript.

This article is supported by the Project from the Education Department of Hainan Province, Project number: Hnky2020ZD-9.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1071907/full#supplementary-material

  • Amani S. (2014). Metacognitive strategy instruction and pre-task planning: Impact on L2 argumentative writing ability . Unpublished doctoral thesis. University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Available at: https://researchspace.auckland.ac.nz/handle/2292/21750
  • Bai B., Guo W. (2021). Motivation and self-regulated strategy use: relationships to primary school students’ English writing in Hong Kong . Lang. Teach. Res. 25 , 378–399. doi: 10.1177/1362168819859921 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bai R., Hu G., Gu P. Y. (2014). The relationship between use of writing strategies and English proficiency in Singapore primary schools . Asia Pac. Educ. Res. 23 , 355–365. doi: 10.1007/s40299-013-0110-0 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown A. (1987). “ Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms ” in Metacognition, Motivation, and Understanding . eds. Weinert F., Kluwe R. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; ), 65–116. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bruning R., Horn C. (2000). Developing motivation to write . Educ. Psychol. 35 , 25–37. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3501_4 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Byrne B. M. (2016). Structural Equation Modelling With AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming (3rd Edn.). New York: Routledge [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chamot A. U., El-Dinary P. B. (1999). Children's learning strategies in immersion classrooms . Mod. Lang. J. 83 , 319–338. doi: 10.1111/0026-7902.00025 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charles M. (1990). Responding to problems in written English using a student self-monitoring technique . ELT J. 44 , 286–293. doi: 10.1093/elt/44.4.286 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chien S.-C. (2012). Students’ use of writing strategies and their English writing achievements in Taiwan . Asia Pacif. J. Educ. 32 , 93–112. doi: 10.1080/02188791.2012.655240 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DiStefano C., Hess B. (2005). Using confirmatory factor analysis for construct. Validation: an empirical review . J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 23 , 225–241. doi: 10.1177/073428290502300303 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dornyei Z. (2010). “ Language aptitude and language learning motivation ” in Continuum Companion to Second Language Acquisition . ed. Macaro E. (London: Continuum; ), 268–299. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Efklides A. (2008). Metacognition: defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation . Eur. Psychol. 13 , 277–287. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.13.4.277 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flavell J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry . Am. Psychol. 34 , 906–911. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flavell J. H. (1985). Cognitive Development (2nd Edn.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice-Hall [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flower L., Hayes J. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing . Coll. Compos. Commun. 32 , 365–387. doi: 10.2307/356600 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Graham S., Harris R. K. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing development . Educ. Psychol. 35 , 3–12. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3501_2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Graham S., MacArthur C., Schwartz S. (1995). The effects of goal setting and procedural facilitation on the revising behavior and writing performance of students with writing and learning problems . J. Educ. Psychol. 87 , 230–240. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.2.230 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes J., Flower L. (1980). The cognition of discovery: defining a rhetorical problem . Coll. Compos. Commun. 31 , 21–32. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu L. T., Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives . Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 6 , 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hyland K. (2007). Genre pedagogy: language, literacy, and L2 writing instruction . J. Second. Lang. Writ. 16 , 148–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2007.07.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lai P. F. (2009). Attitudes of adult Chinese-background learners and an ESL teacher towards L1 use in an Australian AMEP class in Australia . Univ. Sydney Papers TESOL 4 , 57–84. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leung C. B., Hicks J. (2014). Writer identity and writing workshop: a future teacher and teacher educator critically reflect . Writ. Pedagog. 6 , 583–605. doi: 10.1558/wap.v6i3.583 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ma J., Teng F. (2021). “ Metacognitive knowledge development of students with differing levels of writing proficiency in a process-oriented course: an action research study ” in Innovative Approaches in Teaching Writing to Chinese Speakers . eds. Reynolds B. L., Teng F. (Berlin: DeGruyter Mouton; ), 92–117. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Macaro E. (2010). “ The relationship between strategic behaviour and language learning success ” in Continuum Companion to Second Language Acquisition . ed. Macaro E. (London: Continuum; ), 268–299. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Magogwe J. M., Oliver R. (2007). The relationship between language learning strategies, proficiency, age and self-efficacy beliefs: a study of language learners in Botswana . System 35 , 338–352. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2007.01.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer C. (1999). Shaping at the point of utterance: an investigation of the composing processes of the deaf student writer . J. Deaf. Stud. Deaf. Educ. 4 , 37–49. doi: 10.1093/deafed/4.1.37, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nguyen L. T. C., Gu Y. (2013). Strategy-based instruction: a learner-focused approach to developing learner autonomy . Lang. Teach. Res. 17 , 9–30. doi: 10.1177/1362168812457528 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nishino T., Atkinson D. (2015). Second-language writing as sociocognitive alignment . J. Second. Lang. Writ. 27 , 37–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2014.11.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oxford R. L. (1990). Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know . New York: Newbury House/Harper & Row [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oxford R. L. (2013). Teaching and Researching Language Learning Strategies (2nd Edn.). Harlow, UK: Pearson [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oxford R. L., Cohen A. D. (1992). Language learning strategies: crucial issues in concept and classification . Appl. Lang. Learn. 3 , 1–35. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oxford R. L., Nyikos M. (1989). Variables affecting choice of language learning strategies by university students . Mod. Lang. J. 73 , 291–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1989.tb06367.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paris S., Winograd P. (1990). “ How metacognition can promote academic learning and instruction ” in Dimensions of Thinking and Cognitive Instruction . eds. Jones B. F., Idol L. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; ), 15–51. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peacock M., Ho B. (2003). Student language learning strategies across eight disciplines . Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 13 , 179–200. doi: 10.1111/1473-4192.00043 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pugalee D. K. (2001). Writing, mathematics, and metacognition: looking for connections through students’ work in mathematical problem solving . Sch. Sci. Math. 101 , 236–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1949-8594.2001.tb18026.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raimes A. (1987). Language proficiency, writing ability, and composing strategies: a study of ESL college student writers . Lang. Learn. 37 , 439–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1987.tb00579.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Riazi A. (1997). Acquiring disciplinary literacy: a social-cognitive analysis of text production and learning among Iranian graduate students of education . J. Second. Lang. Writ. 6 , 105–137. doi: 10.1016/S1060-3743(97)90030-8 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruan Z. (2014). Metacognitive awareness of EFL student writers in a Chinese ELT context . Lang. Aware. 23 , 76–91. doi: 10.1080/09658416.2013.863901 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schraw G. A. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness . Instr. Sci. 26 , 113–125. doi: 10.1023/A:1003044231033 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schraw G. A. (2001). “ Promoting general metacognitive awareness ” in Metacognition in Learning and Instruction: Theory, Research and Practice . ed. Hartman H. J. (London: Springer; ), 3–16. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schraw G., Dennison R. S. (1994). Assessing metacognitive awareness . Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19 , 460–475. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1994.1033 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schraw G., Moshman D. (1995). Metacognitive theories . Educ. Psychol. Rev. 7 , 351–371. doi: 10.1007/BF02212307 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tabachnick B. G., Fidell L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics . Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F. (2016). Immediate and delayed effects of embedded metacognitive instruction on Chinese EFL students’ English writing and regulation of cognition . Think. Skills Creat. 22 , 289–302. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2016.06.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F. (2019). The role of metacognitive knowledge and regulation in mediating university EFL learners’ writing performance . Innov. Lang. Learn. Teach. 14 , 436–450. doi: 10.1080/17501229.2019.1615493 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F. (2020). Tertiary-level students' English writing performance and metacognitive awareness: a group metacognitive support perspective . Scand. J. Educ. Res. 64 , 551–568. doi: 10.1080/00313831.2019.1595712 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F., Huang J. (2019). Predictive effects of writing strategies for self-regulated learning on secondary school learners’ EFL writing proficiency . TESOL Q. 53 , 232–247. doi: 10.1002/tesq.462 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F., Qin C., Wang C. (2022). Validation of metacognitive academic writing strategies and the predictive effects on academic writing performance in a foreign language context . Metacogn. Learn. 17 , 167–190. doi: 10.1007/s11409-021-09278-4, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F., Wang C., Zhang L. J. (2022). Assessing self-regulatory writing strategies and their predictive effects on young EFL learners’ writing performance . Assess. Writ. 51 :100573. doi: 10.1016/j.asw.2021.100573 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F., Yue M. (2022). Metacognitive writing strategies, critical thinking skills, and academic writing performance: a structural equation modeling approach . Metacogn. Learn . doi: 10.1007/s11409-022-09328-5 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng L. S., Zhang L. J. (2016). A questionnaire-based validation of. Multidimensional models of self-regulated learning strategies . Mod. Lang. J. 100 , 674–701. doi: 10.1111/modl.12339 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng F., Zhang L. J. (2021). Development of children’s metacognitive knowledge, and reading and writing proficiency in English as a foreign language: longitudinal data using multilevel models . Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 91 , 1202–1230. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12413, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Teng L. S., Zhang L. J. (2022). Can self-regulation be transferred to second/foreign language learning and teaching? Current status, controversies, and future directions . Appl. Linguis. 43 , 587–595. doi: 10.1093/applin/amab032 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wenden A. L. (1991). Learner Strategies for Learner Autonomy: Planning and Implementing Learner Training for Language Learners . Cambridge: Prentice Hall International [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wenden A. L. (1998). Metacognitive knowledge and language learning . Appl. Linguist. 19 , 515–537. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wolters C. A. (1999). The relation between high school students’ motivational regulation and their use of learning strategies, effort, and classroom performance . Learn. Individ. Differ. 11 , 281–299. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80004-1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wu Y. L. (2008). Language learning strategies used by students at different proficiency levels . Asian EFL J. 10 , 75–95. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang L. J., Qin L. (2018). “ Validating a questionnaire on EFL writers’ metacognitive awareness of writing strategies in multimedia environments ” in Metacognition in Language Learning and Teaching . eds. Haukås Å., Bjørke C., Dypedahl M. (New York: Routledge; ), 157–178. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang D., Zhang L. J. (2019). “ Metacognition and self-regulated learning (SRL) in second/foreign language teaching ” in Second Handbook of English Language Teaching (Singapore: Springer; ), 883–897. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmerman B. J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: an overview . Theory Pract. 41 , 64–70. doi: 10.1207/s15430421tip4102_2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmerman B. J., Risemberg R. (1997). Becoming a self-regulated writer: a social cognitive perspective . Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 22 , 73–101. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1997.0919 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Marilyn Price-Mitchell Ph.D.

What Is Metacognition? How Does It Help Us Think?

Metacognitive strategies like self-reflection empower students for a lifetime..

Posted October 9, 2020 | Reviewed by Abigail Fagan

Siphotography/Deposit Photos

Metacognition is a high order thinking skill that is emerging from the shadows of academia to take its rightful place in classrooms around the world. As online classrooms extend into homes, this is an important time for parents and teachers to understand metacognition and how metacognitive strategies affect learning. These skills enable children to become better thinkers and decision-makers.

Metacognition: The Neglected Skill Set for Empowering Students is a new research-based book by educational consultants Dr. Robin Fogarty and Brian Pete that not only gets to the heart of why metacognition is important but gives teachers and parents insightful strategies for teaching metacognition to children from kindergarten through high school. This article summarizes several concepts from their book and shares three of their thirty strategies to strengthen metacognition.

What Is Metacognition?

Metacognition is the practice of being aware of one’s own thinking. Some scholars refer to it as “thinking about thinking.” Fogarty and Pete give a great everyday example of metacognition:

Think about the last time you reached the bottom of a page and thought to yourself, “I’m not sure what I just read.” Your brain just became aware of something you did not know, so instinctively you might reread the last sentence or rescan the paragraphs of the page. Maybe you will read the page again. In whatever ways you decide to capture the missing information, this momentary awareness of knowing what you know or do not know is called metacognition.

When we notice ourselves having an inner dialogue about our thinking and it prompts us to evaluate our learning or problem-solving processes, we are experiencing metacognition at work. This skill helps us think better, make sound decisions, and solve problems more effectively. In fact, research suggests that as a young person’s metacognitive abilities increase, they achieve at higher levels.

Fogarty and Pete outline three aspects of metacognition that are vital for children to learn: planning, monitoring, and evaluation. They convincingly argue that metacognition is best when it is infused in teaching strategies rather than taught directly. The key is to encourage students to explore and question their own metacognitive strategies in ways that become spontaneous and seemingly unconscious .

Metacognitive skills provide a basis for broader, psychological self-awareness , including how children gain a deeper understanding of themselves and the world around them.

Metacognitive Strategies to Use at Home or School

Fogarty and Pete successfully demystify metacognition and provide simple ways teachers and parents can strengthen children’s abilities to use these higher-order thinking skills. Below is a summary of metacognitive strategies from the three areas of planning, monitoring, and evaluation.

1. Planning Strategies

As students learn to plan, they learn to anticipate the strengths and weaknesses of their ideas. Planning strategies used to strengthen metacognition help students scrutinize plans at a time when they can most easily be changed.

One of ten metacognitive strategies outlined in the book is called “Inking Your Thinking.” It is a simple writing log that requires students to reflect on a lesson they are about to begin. Sample starters may include: “I predict…” “A question I have is…” or “A picture I have of this is…”

Writing logs are also helpful in the middle or end of assignments. For example, “The homework problem that puzzles me is…” “The way I will solve this problem is to…” or “I’m choosing this strategy because…”

2. Monitoring Strategies

Monitoring strategies used to strengthen metacognition help students check their progress and review their thinking at various stages. Different from scrutinizing, this strategy is reflective in nature. It also allows for adjustments while the plan, activity, or assignment is in motion. Monitoring strategies encourage recovery of learning, as in the example cited above when we are reading a book and notice that we forgot what we just read. We can recover our memory by scanning or re-reading.

One of many metacognitive strategies shared by Fogarty and Pete, called the “Alarm Clock,” is used to recover or rethink an idea once the student realizes something is amiss. The idea is to develop internal signals that sound an alarm. This signal prompts the student to recover a thought, rework a math problem, or capture an idea in a chart or picture. Metacognitive reflection involves thinking about “What I did,” then reviewing the pluses and minuses of one’s action. Finally, it means asking, “What other thoughts do I have” moving forward?

metacognitive knowledge essay

Teachers can easily build monitoring strategies into student assignments. Parents can reinforce these strategies too. Remember, the idea is not to tell children what they did correctly or incorrectly. Rather, help children monitor and think about their own learning. These are formative skills that last a lifetime.

3. Evaluation Strategies

According to Fogarty and Pete, the evaluation strategies of metacognition “are much like the mirror in a powder compact. Both serve to magnify the image, allow for careful scrutiny, and provide an up-close and personal view. When one opens the compact and looks in the mirror, only a small portion of the face is reflected back, but that particular part is magnified so that every nuance, every flaw, and every bump is blatantly in view.” Having this enlarged view makes inspection much easier.

When students inspect parts of their work, they learn about the nuances of their thinking processes. They learn to refine their work. They grow in their ability to apply their learning to new situations. “Connecting Elephants” is one of many metacognitive strategies to help students self-evaluate and apply their learning.

In this exercise, the metaphor of three imaginary elephants is used. The elephants are walking together in a circle, connected by the trunk and tail of another elephant. The three elephants represent three vital questions: 1) What is the big idea? 2) How does this connect to other big ideas? 3) How can I use this big idea? Using the image of a “big idea” helps students magnify and synthesize their learning. It encourages them to think about big ways their learning can be applied to new situations.

Metacognition and Self-Reflection

Reflective thinking is at the heart of metacognition. In today’s world of constant chatter, technology and reflective thinking can be at odds. In fact, mobile devices can prevent young people from seeing what is right before their eyes.

John Dewey, a renowned psychologist and education reformer, claimed that experiences alone were not enough. What is critical is an ability to perceive and then weave meaning from the threads of our experiences.

The function of metacognition and self-reflection is to make meaning. The creation of meaning is at the heart of what it means to be human.

Everyone can help foster self-reflection in young people.

Marilyn Price-Mitchell Ph.D.

Marilyn Price-Mitchell, Ph.D., is an Institute for Social Innovation Fellow at Fielding Graduate University and author of Tomorrow’s Change Makers.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Teletherapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Therapy Center NEW
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

March 2024 magazine cover

Understanding what emotional intelligence looks like and the steps needed to improve it could light a path to a more emotionally adept world.

  • Coronavirus Disease 2019
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience
  • International Education Studies
  • Vol. 7, No. 13 (2014)

The Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Essay Writing among High School Students

  •   Shahlan Surat    
  •   Saemah Rahman    
  •   Zamri Mahamod    
  •   Saadiah Kummin    

This paper report part of a bigger project aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of metacognitive strategies on students’ performance in essay writing. The aspects of metacognitive strategies considered in this study include the use of declarative knowledge, conditional knowledge, and procedural knowledge. The focus of this paper is on the use of metacognitive strategies during the writing activity. Before the intervention process, the participants were given a task to write an essay and after that they were asked to do metacognitive reflection. Data were analyzed using content analysis procedure. The respondents consisted of 18 secondary school students from poor urban community. The data revealed that ten participants did not use declarative knowledge namely: i) did not make the outline of the essay before writing, ii) did not identify keywords that represent the requirement of the question, and iii) how to expand ideas. These results indicate that students did not possess enough declarative knowledge about writing. The study also found that 11 respondents did not use declarative knowledge in the attempt to expand ideas. In terms of conditional knowledge, nine of the respondents still could not identify when and why certain strategies should be used. Whereas in terms of procedural knowledge, the study showed that all the participants did not show the use of important steps needed in writing a good essay. In conclusion, this study provides evidence on the need of an intervention or teaching modules to help improve students writing skills.

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • DOI: 10.5539/ies.v7n13p212

metacognitive knowledge essay

  • ISSN(Print): 1913-9020
  • ISSN(Online): 1913-9039
  • Started: 2008
  • Frequency: bimonthly

Journal Metrics

h-index : 62 i10-index: 604

  • Academic Journals Database
  • AcademicKeys
  • BASE (Bielefeld Academic Search Engine)
  • Berkeley Library
  • CNKI Scholar
  • Copyright Clearance Center
  • DESY Publication Database
  • DTU Library
  • Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
  • Educational Research Abstracts
  • Electronic Journals Library
  • Elektronische Zeitschriftenbibliothek (EZB)
  • Excellence in Research for Australia (ERA)
  • Genamics JournalSeek
  • GETIT@YALE (Yale University Library)
  • Ghent University Library
  • Harvard Library
  • Jisc Library Hub Discover
  • JournalGuide
  • JournalTOCs
  • LSE Library
  • Microsoft Academic
  • Norwegian Centre for Research Data (NSD)
  • Open J-Gate
  • PKP Open Archives Harvester
  • Polska Bibliografia Naukowa
  • Qualis/CAPES
  • ResearchGate
  • SHERPA/RoMEO
  • Southwest-German Union Catalogue
  • Standard Periodical Directory
  • Stanford Libraries
  • Technische Informationsbibliothek (TIB)
  • The Keepers Registry
  • UCR Library
  • Universe Digital Library
  • UoS Library
  • USask Library
  • Chris Lee Editorial Assistant
  • [email protected]
  • Journal Home
  • Announcements
  • Editorial Team
  • Order Hard Copies

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Students' metacognitive knowledge of learning-strategy effectiveness and their recall of teachers' strategy instructions provisionally accepted.

  • 1 School of Natural Sciences and Health, Tallinn University, Estonia
  • 2 Tallinn University, Estonia

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

This study aimed to investigate students' metacognitive knowledge and reported use of surface and deep learning strategies. It also explored the extent to which students recall their teachers' recommendations for learning strategies and the relationship between these recollections and students' knowledge and reported use of strategies. A scenario-based questionnaire was used to set a learning goal in the area of biology. Students' metacognitive knowledge was assessed through perceived effectiveness and reported use of learning strategies. Additionally, open-ended questions allowed students to recall and report recommendations given by their teachers. We used personcentered methods to explore whether different types of recollections were related to reported strategy use. Among students who recollected that their teachers have recommended deep learning strategies, it was typical to value deep strategies higher than surface strategies and report using deep strategies. Also, it was atypical among those students to value surface level strategies and not use deep strategies.

Keywords: metacognitive knowledge, Learning Strategies, Teacher instruction, Configural frequency analysis (CFA), Strategy effectiveness

Received: 04 Oct 2023; Accepted: 22 Apr 2024.

Copyright: © 2024 Olop, Granström and Kikas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Mx. Joosep Olop, School of Natural Sciences and Health, Tallinn University, Tallin, Estonia

People also looked at

IMAGES

  1. Metacognition Essay Free Essay Example

    metacognitive knowledge essay

  2. Metacognition

    metacognitive knowledge essay

  3. (PDF) The Impact of Metacognitive Learning Techniques on Learners

    metacognitive knowledge essay

  4. Psychological Components of Metacognition Research Paper Example

    metacognitive knowledge essay

  5. Metacognitive Theory

    metacognitive knowledge essay

  6. Metacognition

    metacognitive knowledge essay

VIDEO

  1. METACOGNITIVE REGULATION

  2. Metacognitive Strategies

  3. Metacognitive Processes

  4. METACOGNITION

  5. Planning Strategies in Metacognitive Regulation

  6. EVALUATION STRATEGIES IN METACOGNITIVE LEARNING

COMMENTS

  1. Metacognition: ideas and insights from neuro- and educational ...

    Metacognition is defined as "thinking about thinking" or the ability to monitor and control one's cognitive processes 1 and plays an important role in learning and education 2,3,4.For ...

  2. Writing for Metacognition: Encouraging thinking about thinking

    Metacognition describes an awareness of this process: the ways we absorb, assimilate, and convey information and participate in knowledge production. A growing body of research shows that as students practice metacognition, they are better able to assess and adapt their facility with the knowledge and skills of a discipline and to transfer ...

  3. Making Metacognition Part of Student Writing

    3. Writing collaboratively. Provide opportunities for students to work on writing assignments together. The students can discuss why they are making the choices they make along the way. Thoughts can be addressed in comments in a Google Doc or on sticky notes placed on the student's paper. 4. Using graphic organizers.

  4. PDF The Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Essay Writing among High School

    1.1 Metacognitive Knowledge In this study, metacognitive strategy refers to the use of metacognitive knowledge namely declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge and conditional knowledge in essay writing. Flavell (1976, 1978, 1979) described metacognitive knowledge as consisting of knowledge or one's belief in basic knowledge about the ...

  5. Implicit Theory of Writing Ability: Relationship to Metacognitive

    Writing an academic essay is a complex and multidimensional process that requires the activation and use of different SRL components (Harris, Santangelo, & Graham, 2010). ... Metacognitive strategy knowledge refers to the knowledge component of metacognition (Flavell, 1979) and describes verbalizable knowledge and awareness of memory ...

  6. Metacognition

    A common assignment in English composition courses is the self-assessment essay in which students apply course criteria to articulate their strengths and weaknesses within single papers or over the course of the semester. ... The Role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching, and assessing. Theory into Practice, 41 (4). 219-225 ...

  7. Validation of metacognitive academic writing strategies and the

    In this academic writing test, learners were required to write an essay on a topic related to medicine and health. The topic was chosen in responding to the institutional advocate of enhancing students' awareness of self-care needs due to the COVID-19 pandemic. ... The metacognitive knowledge dimension comprises declarative knowledge ...

  8. Metacognition in Academic Writing: Learning Dimensions

    Metacognition refers to the unique human ability to reflect on one's own knowledge and thinking. This ability is crucial for learning and agency: metacognition allows us to assess what we know and do not know (including relevant previous experiences), set realistic goals, plan, monitor, and evaluate our performance. You have full access to ...

  9. Metacognitive writing strategies, critical thinking skills, and

    Metacognitive knowledge is also connected with writing performance (Graham, 2006; Teng & Zhang, 2021). ... Section 4 focuses on essay writing; the students were required to write an essay of approximately 150 words for a research topic based on the provided information. For example, students were required to synthesize possible reasons after ...

  10. [PDF] The Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Essay Writing among High

    This paper report part of a bigger project aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of metacognitive strategies on students' performance in essay writing. The aspects of metacognitive strategies considered in this study include the use of declarative knowledge, conditional knowledge, and procedural knowledge. The focus of this paper is on the use of metacognitive strategies during the writing ...

  11. Fostering Metacognition to Support Student Learning and Performance

    Metacognition is awareness and control of thinking for learning. Strong metacognitive skills have the power to impact student learning and performance. While metacognition can develop over time with practice, many students struggle to meaningfully engage in metacognitive processes. In an evidence-based teaching guide associated with this paper ...

  12. Thinking About Thinking: Metacognition

    Metacognitive Knowledge - Reflecting on What We Know. Flavell (1979) ... Well, if I'm learning to write an essay, and let's say I write two essays, and I get an A from you on one of them and a C on the other, and the teacher says "See, you CAN write a good essay, do more of what you did on the A essay, than what you did on the C essay

  13. Upper-Elementary Students' Metacognitive Knowledge about Writing and

    The metawriting task was coded for eight dimensions of metacognitive knowledge. Students' writing performance was assessed via additional 30-minute prompts—two narrative, one informative, two persuasive—and evaluated for quality and length using automated essay scoring.

  14. PDF Students' Metacognitive Awareness and Its Impact on Writing Skill

    procedural knowledge so that they are not able to show the right steps in writing (Surat, Rahman, Mahamod, & Kummin, 2014). These findings prove that metacognitive knowledge is an important aspect that can help students achieve success in writing. Success in writing is greatly influenced by the metacognitive knowledge base, such as personal ...

  15. The Instruction of Writing Strategies: The Effect of the Metacognitive

    An assessment of the metacognitive knowledge of Botswana ESL university student writers. Educational Research and Reviews, 8, 1988-1995. Google Scholar. ... Harris K. R., Graham S. (1998). Self-regulated strategy development and the writing process: Effects on essay writing and attributions. Exceptional Children, 64, 295-311. Crossref. ISI.

  16. A questionnaire-based validation of metacognitive strategies in writing

    When composing their essays, lower-level writers often experienced difficulty in transferring ideas to paper during the planning, monitoring, and self-evaluating stages. ... Teng F. (2021). " Metacognitive knowledge development of students with differing levels of writing proficiency in a process-oriented course: an action research study ...

  17. (PDF) The Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Essay ...

    Similarly, Yang & Zhang (2002) investigated the role of metacognitive knowledge in Chinese students' essays. The students were given the task of writing an essay and then complete a questionnaire.

  18. ERIC

    This paper reports part of a bigger project aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of metacognitive strategies on students' performance in essay writing. The aspects of metacognitive strategies considered in this study include the use of declarative knowledge, conditional knowledge, and procedural knowledge. The focus of this paper is on the use of metacognitive strategies during the writing ...

  19. (PDF) Metacognition

    Metacognition is a type of individual difference that can be both trait-like and state-like (Sato 2022). Researchers have found that metacognition plays a role in L2 development (Efklides 2006 ...

  20. What Is Metacognition? How Does It Help Us Think?

    Metacognition is the practice of being aware of one's own thinking. Some scholars refer to it as "thinking about thinking.". Fogarty and Pete give a great everyday example of metacognition ...

  21. PDF Metacognitive Teaching and Learning Activities

    Pre-Class Knowledge Survey . ... 1. At the beginning of a term, assign a short essay (~750 words) prompting students to identify their goals for ... 1. Include several metacognitive questions about how a student prepared for the exam at the end of an exam. 2. Students answer these questions after finishing the formal exam.

  22. The Use of Metacognitive Knowledge in Essay Writing among High School

    This paper report part of a bigger project aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of metacognitive strategies on students' performance in essay writing. The aspects of metacognitive strategies considered in this study include the use of declarative knowledge, conditional knowledge, and procedural knowledge.

  23. Metacognition

    Metacognition is an awareness of one's thought processes and an understanding of the patterns behind them. The term comes from the root word meta, meaning "beyond", or "on top of". Metacognition can take many forms, such as reflecting on one's ways of thinking and knowing when and how to use particular strategies for problem-solving. There are generally two components of metacognition: (1 ...

  24. Frontiers

    This study aimed to investigate students' metacognitive knowledge and reported use of surface and deep learning strategies. It also explored the extent to which students recall their teachers' recommendations for learning strategies and the relationship between these recollections and students' knowledge and reported use of strategies. A scenario-based questionnaire was used to set a learning ...