BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

The use of new technologies for improving reading comprehension.

\r\nAgnese Capodieci*

  • 1 Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padua, Italy
  • 2 Azienda Sociosanitaria Ligure 5 Spezzino, La Spezia, Italy

Since the introduction of writing systems, reading comprehension has always been a foundation for achievement in several areas within the educational system, as well as a prerequisite for successful participation in most areas of adult life. The increased availability of technologies and web-based resources can be a really valid support, both in the educational and clinical field, to devise training activities that can also be carried out remotely. There are studies in current literature that has examined the efficacy of internet-based programs for reading comprehension for children with reading comprehension difficulties but almost none considered distance rehabilitation programs. The present paper reports data concerning a distance program Cloze , developed in Italy, for improving language and reading comprehension. Twenty-eight children from 3rd to 6th grade with comprehension difficulties were involved. These children completed the distance program for 15–20 min for at least three times a week for about 4 months. The program was presented separately to each child, with a degree of difficulty adapted to his/her characteristics. Text reading comprehension (assessed distinguishing between narrative and informative texts) increased after intervention. These findings have clinical and educational implications as they suggest that it is possible to promote reading comprehension with a distance individualized program, avoiding the need for the child displacements, necessary for reaching a rehabilitation center.

Introduction

Reading comprehension is a fundamental cognitive ability for children, that supports school achievement and successively participation in most areas of adult life ( Hulme and Snowling, 2011 ). Therefore, children with learning disabilities (LD) and special educational needs who show difficulties in text comprehension, sometimes also in association with other problems, may have an increased risk of life and school failure ( Woolley, 2011 ). Reading comprehension is, indeed, a complex cognitive ability which involves not only linguistic (e.g., vocabulary, grammatical knowledge), but also cognitive (such as working memory, De Beni and Palladino, 2000 ), and metacognitive skills (both for the aspects of knowledge and control, Channa et al., 2015 ), and, more specifically, higher order comprehension skills such as the generation of inferences ( Oakhill et al., 2003 ).

Recently, due to the diffusion of technology in many fields of daily life, text comprehension at school, at home during homework, and at work is based on an increasing number of digital reading devices (computers and laptops, e-books, and tablet devices) that can become a fundamental support to improve traditional reading comprehension and learning skills (e.g., inference generation).

Some authors contrasted in children with typical development the effects of the technological interface on reading comprehension vs printed texts ( Kerr and Symons, 2006 ; Rideout et al., 2010 ; Mangen et al., 2013 ; Singer and Alexander, 2017 ; Delgado et al., 2018 ). Results were consistent and showed a worse comprehension performance in screen texts compared to printed texts for children ( Mangen et al., 2013 ; Delgado et al., 2018 ) and adolescents who nonetheless showed a preference for digital texts compared to printed texts ( Singer and Alexander, 2017 ). Regarding children with learning problems, only few studies considered the differences between printed texts and digital devices ( Chen, 2009 ; Gonzalez, 2014 ; Krieger, 2017 ) finding no significant differences, suggesting that the use of compensative digital tools for children with a learning difficulty could be a valid alternative with respect to the traditional written texts in facilitating their academic and work performance. This conclusion is also supported by the results of a meta-analysis ( Moran et al., 2008 ), regarding the use of digital tools and learning environments for enhancing literacy acquisition in middle school students, which demonstrates that technology can improve reading comprehension.

Different procedures and abilities are targeted in the international literature concerning computerized training programs for reading comprehension. In particular, various studies include activities promoting cognitive (e.g., vocabulary, inference making) and metacognitive (e.g., the use of strategies, comprehension monitoring, and identification of relevant parts in a text) components of reading comprehension. Table 1 reports the list of papers proposing computerized training programs with a summary of the findings encountered. Participants involved cover different ages and school grades, the majority belonging to middle school and high school. The general outcome of the studies is positive due to a significant improvement in comprehension skills after the training program with long-lasting effects also during follow-up; indeed, the majority of participants involved in training programs outperformed their peers assigned to comparison groups and maintained their improvements. Specifically, several studies ( O’Reilly et al., 2004 ; Magliano et al., 2005 ; McNamara et al., 2006 ) used the iSTART program with adolescents and young adults. This program promotes self-explanation, prior knowledge and reading strategies to enhance understanding of descriptive scientific texts. Results demonstrated that students who followed the iSTART program received more benefits than their peers, improving self-explanation and summarization. Additionally, strategic knowledge was a relevant factor for the outcome in comprehension tasks including multiple choice questions: students who already possessed good strategic knowledge improved their accuracy when answering to bridging inference questions, whereas students with low strategic knowledge became more accurate with text-based questions. Another program, ITSS, was used with younger students ( Meyer et al., 2011 ; Wijekumar et al., 2012 , 2013 , 2017 ), with the objective to support activities based on identifying main parts and key words in a text and classifying information in a hierarchical order. Positive outcomes were found also with such program since students who followed the ITSS program significantly improved text comprehension compared to their peers in the control group.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Synthesis of the main results of the computerized training programs on comprehension present in the literature.

Although most of the literature deals with typical development, also cases of students with learning difficulties were considered. For example, Potocki et al. (2013) (see also Potocki et al., 2015 ) examined the effects of two different computerized programs with specific aims: one focusing on comprehension features, such as inference making and the analysis of text structure, the other considering decoding skills. Both training programs brought some benefits to reading comprehension, however larger effects were found with the program focused on comprehension with long-lasting effects in listening and reading comprehension (see also Kleinsz et al., 2017 ). Studies by Johnson-Glenberg (2005) and Kim et al. (2006) , using respectively the programs 3D Readers and CACSR, were able to promote reading comprehension abilities in middle school students through metacognitive activities. Thanks to these programs students also became more aware of reading strategies and implemented them more successfully during text comprehension. In particular, a study by Niedo et al. (2014) , obtained positive results on silent reading in a small group of children struggling with reading using the “cloze” procedure. This procedure proposes exercises in which parts of a text, typically words, are missing and participants are required to complete the text guessing what is missing.

Thus, computerized programs generally seem to improve reading comprehension skills. However, it should be noticed that, in most cases, students were trained at school, without the personalized support of a clinician taking into consideration the cognitive and psychological needs of the child. In particular, to our knowledge, no program examined the effects of an internet-based distance reading comprehension program which allows the child to be trained at home in a personalized way. A useful aspect of an internet-based distance training is that the psychologist can monitor with the application ( app ) the child’s results and activities and write him/her some motivational messages, reducing the attritions present in programs carried out at home with the only supervision of parents. Literature concerning distance trainings is still rare, however, some evidence suggests that these programs may represent a good integration to other types of intervention, usually carried out at school, in a rehabilitation center or at home (e.g., Mich et al., 2013 ).

Therefore, despite still preliminary, we think that it is relevant to present data about a distance program developed in Italy named Cloze ( Cornoldi and Bertolo, 2013 ), devised for rehabilitation purposes but with potential implication also for educational contexts. Cloze has been developed to promote inferential abilities both at a sentence- and discourse-level using the “cloze” procedure. Several findings in the literature demonstrate that abilities, such as anticipating text parts and inference making, bring improvements in text comprehension (e.g., Yuill and Oakhill, 1988 ) and it has been shown that one way to promote inferential competences is to improve the ability to predict parts of the text that are missing or that follow, considering the available information: the “cloze” technique appears to be one of the most successful ways for this purpose (e.g., Greene, 2001 ).

In the current study the effectiveness of this training program has been tested on a clinical population who exhibited, for various reasons, difficulties in reading comprehension. Participants were 28 children (16 male and 12 female) attending a private practice for learning difficulties in the city of La Spezia, in the north-west of Italy, from 3rd to 6th school grade (5 of 3rd, 9 of 4th, 11 of 5th and 3 of 6th grade), with a mean age of children of M = 9.79 years (SD = 1.03). Seventeen children had a current or past speech disorder: of these children 10 also had a LD (Learning Disabilities) and one was bilingual (speech problems were not due to bilingualism). The other 11 children had a LD or important learning difficulties, and one of them had also ADHD (Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder). For the goals of the study, all these children were considered together as they all presented a severe reading comprehension difficulty as reported by parents and teachers and confirmed by the initial assessment.

All children had received a comprehensive psychological assessment (see Table 2 ), adapted to their particular needs and ages. In particular all children had an IQ >80 assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003 ) and did not have anxiety disorders, mood affective disorders or other developmental disorders, with the exception of the cases with language disorder and the case with ADHD. Children were not receiving any additional treatment, including medication. Written consent was obtained from the children’s parents in the context of the private practice.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Main characteristics of the sample in terms of reading and cognitive abilities.

Materials and Methods

Pre-/post-test assessment and procedure of the training.

Each child started a training program through the distance rehabilitation platform Ridinet, using the Cloze app, after the assessment of learning and cognitive abilities, including comprehension assessment with two texts, one narrative and one informative ( Cornoldi and Carretti, 2016 ; Cornoldi et al., 2017 ). Connection to the Ridinet web site was required in order to access to the app, three or four times a week for more or less 15/20 min. The period of use was of 3 months for 6 children and 4 months for 22 children. After this period children’s comprehension was assessed again. Additionally, some questions were asked to parents and children about the app’s utility and pleasantness. In particular, children were asked: “Do you think the program helped you improve your text comprehension skills?,” “Did you like doing this program instead of the same exercises on paper?”; and parents were asked: “Was it difficult to start the Cloze activities on days when it had to be done?,” “Compared to the beginning of the treatment, how do you currently judge the ability of your child to understand the texts?”. For all questions, except the last one, the answer had to be given on a 5-point scale with 1 = not at all, 2 = a little, 3 = enough, 4 = very, 5 = very much. For the last question the answer changed on a 4-point scale with 1 = got worse, 2 = unchanged, 3 = slightly improved, and 4 = greatly improved.

Comprehension Tasks

Reading comprehension was assessed with two texts, the first narrative and the other informative, taken from Italian batteries for the assessment of reading ( Cornoldi and Carretti, 2016 ; Cornoldi et al., 2017 ). The texts range between 226 and 455 words in length, and their length increases with school grade (in order to have texts and questions matching the degrees of expertise at different grades the batteries include a different pair of texts for each grade). Students read the text in silence at their own pace, then answer a variable number of multiple-choice questions (depending on school grade), choosing one of four possible answers. There is no time limit, and students can reread the text whenever they wish. The final score is calculated as the total number of correct answers for each text. Alpha coefficients, as reported by the manuals, range between 0.61 and 0.83. For the purposes of the study we decided to use the same two comprehension texts, at pre-test and post-test, as the procedure offered the opportunity of directly examining and showing to parents changes in comprehension and previous evidence had shown the absence of relevant retest effects with this material in a retest carried out after 3 months ( Viola and Carretti, 2019 ).

Distance Rehabilitation Program: Cloze

Cloze ( Cornoldi and Bertolo, 2013 ) is an app for the promotion of text comprehension with the specific aim to recover processes of lexical and semantic inference. At each work session the child works with texts that lack words and must complete the empty spaces by choosing the correct alternative from those automatically proposed by the app, so that the text becomes congruent. The program is adaptive, as text complexity and proportion of missing words vary according to the previous level of response, and is designed for children who have weaknesses in written text comprehension, mainly due to poor skills in lexical and semantic inferential processes. The app also allows to enhance a set of language skills (phonology, syntax, semantics) which contribute to ensuring the fluidity of text and production processing. The recommended age range for the use of this program is between 7 and 14 years. In this study the semantic mode (only content words may be missing and no syntactic cues can be used for deciding between the alternatives) was proposed to 21 children and the syntactic mode (where all words may be missing) to 7 children. The mode type selected for each child depends from the performance at pre-test and diagnosis. A clinician, co-author of the present study (LB), monitored the child’s results and activities with the app and sent him/her from time to time some motivational messages. The motivational messages were typically sent once a week for congratulating with children for the work done and check with him/her possible problems emerged. Training lasted from 3 to 4 months and involved between 3 and 4 sessions of 15–20 min per week. The variation in duration depended on the decision of each individual family. In fact, children were required to use the software for about 4 months or in any case for a minimum period of 3 months (choice made by six families).

Effects on Reading Comprehension of Cloze Training

All analyses were carried out with SPSS 25 ( IBM Corp, 2017 ). A preliminary analysis found that all the examined variables met the assumptions of normality (K-S between 0.106 and 0.143, p > 0.05). Then, we compared the reading comprehension performance of children before and after the computerized training with Cloze . For this analysis, a repeated measure Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted on comprehension scores to examine the differences in the whole group of children between the scores obtained before and after the training. A significant difference was found for both comprehension texts [ F (1,27) = 22.37, p < 0.001, η 2 p = 0.453 and F (1,27) = 38.90, p < 0.001, η 2 p = 0.599, respectively]. Possible differences between the two training modalities (semantic vs syntactic) and between different training periods (3 months vs 4 months) were then analyzed; no significant differences emerged between groups in both cases [ F (1,27) < 1].

Secondly, to analyze the role of individual differences at pre-test, the standardized training gain score (STG; Jaeggi et al., 2011 ) – computed by subtracting post-test score minus pre-test score, divided by the SD of the pre-test – was calculated for the two texts comprehension. Pearson correlations were computed between the STG and the variable collected at pre-test (reading speed and errors, WISC IV – Full scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory and Processing Speed indexes). The only significant correlation was between STG of the narrative text and Verbal Comprehension Index of the WISC-IV Scale ( r = 0.38, p = 0.048). Finally, individual improvements from pre- to post-test were also confirmed considering changes in performance in terms of standard deviation in relations to norms (provided by the manual). Table 3 shows the number of children for each comprehension text who improved their performance moving from a performance at least 2 standard deviations or between 1 and 2 negative standard deviations under the mean to a performance above one negative standard deviation.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Changes in performance in relations to norms (provided by the manual) after the training program Cloze.

Perceived Utility, Pleasantness, Parents and Child’s Improvements of Cloze

Results concerning the answers of parents and children about utility, pleasantness and self-perceived efficacy of the app, were also analyzed. At the first question, addressing children’s perceived improvement in comprehension skills, more than half of the sample chose the alternatives “very” or “very much” (15 “very” and 5 “very much”), only 1 child answered “a little” and the others chose “enough.” At the second question, about the pleasure of doing this kind of activity instead of pen and paper activities, all children answered “very” or “very much.” Concerning parents’ questions, at the first question about the difficulty to start the Cloze activity, only one parent answered “enough,” a quarter of the sample chose “a little” (seven families) and all the other 20 families chose the alternative “not at all.” At the last question about the perceived training efficacy on their child’s performance, the large majority of the families chose “slightly improved” or “greatly improved” and only three parents thought their children’s ability had remained unchanged. However, no correlations between parents and child’s perceived improvements and STG in reading comprehension were found.

The present study examined the effects of the use of Cloze , a distance rehabilitation program focused on inference skills, for improving reading comprehension, on the basis of the hypothesis that, being inference making related to reading comprehension at different ages (e.g., Oakhill and Cain, 2012 ), positive effects of the training activities on reading comprehension should be found.

Concerning the efficacy of computer-assisted training programs, literature highlights that many training programs are devised for an educational context. Results are generally encouraging with positive effects on reading comprehension, measured with materials different from those practiced during the training. However, few studies analyzed the efficacy in children with specific reading comprehension problems, and no studies considered the possibility of carrying out a training at home under the distance supervision of an expert. The latter characteristics are those that make the Cloze peculiar compared to the existent literature. Cloze is indeed based on a rehabilitation online platform which allows the child to complete personalized training activities several times a week, without moving from his/her home, and concurrently enabling the clinician to monitor the child’s progress or manage activities’ characteristics. The advantage of this procedure is twofold: on one hand it increases the potential number of training sessions per week, on the other hand it permits to save the necessary time to reach the center for rehabilitation and to reduce the costs of the intervention.

The preliminary data on Cloze were generally positive: children, working on either two slightly different versions of the same program, showed a generalized improvement in reading comprehension tasks and, together with their families, expressed appreciation for the pleasantness and the efficacy of the program. Encouraging results emerged also from the analysis of individual improvements referring to normative scores, as reported in Table 3 : most of the children’s performance migrated from a highly negative level to an average level.

It is noticeable that the efficacy of the training was assessed with materials different from those practiced during the training sessions, since reading comprehension tasks required to read a paper text and complete a series of multiple-choice questions. In future studies it would be interesting to analyze the effects of the program on skills known to be related to text comprehension, such as vocabulary or comprehension monitoring, for example. There is good reason to believe that since these variables are highly predictive of comprehension skills (and given that training in these skills sometimes improve comprehension; e.g., Beck et al., 1982 ; see also Hulme and Snowling, 2011 ), training that specifically targets comprehension might, in turn, lead to improvements in vocabulary or comprehension monitoring skills. Further studies are needed to explore this hypothesis.

A second relevant finding of the present study is the presence of a positive correlation between the gain obtained in one of the reading comprehension text (the narrative one) and the Verbal Comprehension Intelligence Quotient (VCIQ) index of the WISC-IV battery, showing that children who started with more resources in verbal intelligence achieved greater improvements in text comprehension at least with one type of text through the Cloze . The activities probably required to develop some kind of strategies, and for this reason students with larger verbal intellectual resources, who were presumably more able to develop new strategies, were more advantaged. Indeed, this amplification effect is usually found when training activities require the development of strategies ( von Bastian and Oberauer, 2014 ). Such result has clinical and educational implications, inviting professionals and teachers to consider children’s starting resources and, if necessary, to combine activities conducted through distance rehabilitation programs with personal intervention sessions that could teach strategies and promote a metacognitive approach to reading comprehension. However, some limitations of the present study must be acknowledged. Firstly, study did not include a control group, therefore findings should be taken with caution, although normative data and previous results obtained with the same test offer support to the robustness of our results and the use of normative data offers a control measure of how reading comprehension skills are acquired in typically developing children without specific training, therefore functioning as a sort of passive control group. Secondly, the treated group, although characterized by a common reading comprehension difficulty, was partly heterogeneous, as children attended different grades and could have different diagnoses. Unfortunately, the limited number of subjects, with the consequence that it was not possible to form groups defined both by the grade and the diagnosis, did not permit to make analyses taking into account the grade and the diagnosis as between-subjects factors. Future studies should examine a more homogeneous population or consider a larger sample of children, giving more information about the efficacy of training in different children population. Additionally, the fact that the treatment was concluded with the post-training assessment did not offer the opportunity to further examine the procedure and maintenance effects with a follow-up. Despite the limitations, this study offers evidence concerning the efficacy of new methods, based on computer-assisted training programs that could be beneficial in training high-level skills such as comprehension and inference generation. Such tools can be extremely worthwhile for struggling readers who may need to receive further attention in mastering higher level reading comprehension.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

AC, CC and BC contributed to the design and implementation of the research. LB provided the data. BC organized the database. AC performed the statistical analysis. ED did the literature research and wrote the section about the review of the literature. AC and BC wrote the other sections. CC contributed to the manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

The present work was carried out within the scope of the research program Dipartimenti di Eccellenza (art.1, commi 314-337 legge 232/2016), which was supported by a grant from MIUR to the Department of General Psychology, University of Padua and partially supported by a grant (PRIN 2015, 2015AR52F9_003) to Cesare Cornoldi funded by the Italian Ministry of Research and Education (MIUR).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., and McKeown, M. G. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 506–521. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Channa, M. A., Nordin, Z. S., Siming, I. A., Chandio, A. A., and Koondher, M. A. (2015). Developing reading comprehension through metacognitive strategies: a review of previous studies. Eng. Lang. Teach. 8, 181–186. doi: 10.5539/elt.v8n8p181

Chen, H. (2009). Online reading comprehension strategies among fifth- and sixth-grade general and special education students. Educ. Res. Perspect. 37, 79–109.

Google Scholar

Cornoldi, C., and Bertolo, L. (2013). Cloze Ridinet. Bologna: Anastasis.

Cornoldi, C., and Carretti, B. (2016). Prove MT-3-Clinica. Firenze: Giunti Edu.

Cornoldi, C., Carretti, B., and Colpo, C. (2017). Prove MT-Kit Scuola. Dalla valutazione degli Apprendimenti di Lettura E Comprensione Al Potenziamento. [MT-Kit for the Assessment In The School. From Reading Assessment To Its Enhancement]. Firenze: Giunti Edu.

Cullen, J. M., Alber-Morgan, S. R., Schnell, S. T., and Wheaton, J. E. (2014). Improving reading skills of students with disabilities using headsprout comprehension. Remed. Spec. Educ. 35, 356–365. doi: 10.1177/0741932514534075

De Beni, R., and Palladino, P. (2000). Intrusion errors in working memory tasks: are they related to reading comprehension ability? Learn. Individ. Differ. 12, 131–143. doi: 10.1016/s1041-6080(01)00033-4

Delgado, P., Vargas, C., Ackerman, R., and Salmerón, L. (2018). Don’t throw away your printed books: a meta-analysis on the effects of reading media on reading comprehension. Educ. Res. Rev. 25, 23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.09.003

Gonzalez, M. (2014). The effect of embedded text-to-speech and vocabulary eBook scaffolds on the comprehension of students with reading disabilities. Intern. J. Spec. Educ. 29, 111–125.

Greene, B. (2001). Testing reading comprehension of theoretical discourse with cloze. J. Res. Read. 24, 82–98. doi: 10.1111/1467-9817.00134

Hulme, C., and Snowling, M. J. (2011). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties: nature, causes, and treatments. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 20, 139–142. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408673

IBM Corp (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., and Shah, P. (2011). Short-and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 10081–10086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103228108

Johnson-Glenberg, M. C. (2005). Web-based training of metacognitive strategies for text comprehension: focus on poor comprehenders. Read. Writ. 18, 755–786. doi: 10.1007/s11145-005-0956-5

Kerr, M. A., and Symons, S. E. (2006). Computerized presentation of text: effects on children’s reading of informational material. Read. Writ. 19, 1–19. doi: 10.1007/s11145-003-8128-y

Kim, A. H., Vaughn, S., Klingner, J. K., Woodruff, A. L., Klein Reutebuch, C., and Kouzekanani, K. (2006). Improving the reading comprehension of middle school students with disabilities through computer-assisted collaborative strategic reading. Remed. Spec. Educ. 27, 235–249. doi: 10.1177/07419325060270040401

Kleinsz, N., Potocki, A., Ecalle, J., and Magnan, A. (2017). Profiles of French poor readers: underlying difficulties and effects of computerized training programs. Learn. Individ. Differ. 57, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.05.009

Krieger, R. (2017). The Effect of Electronic Text Reading on Reading Comprehension Scores of Students with Disabilities. Master thesis, Governors State University, Park, IL.

Leong, C. K. (1992). Enhancing reading comprehension with text-to-speech (DECtalk) computer system. Read. Writ. 4, 205–217. doi: 10.1007/bf01027492

Magliano, J. P., Todaro, S., Millis, K., Wiemer-Hastings, K., Kim, H. J., and McNamara, D. S. (2005). Changes in reading strategies as a function of reading training: a comparison of live and computerized training. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 32, 185–208. doi: 10.2190/1ln8-7bqe-8tn0-m91l

Mangen, A., Walgermo, B. R., and Brønnick, K. (2013). Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: effects on reading comprehension. Intern. J. Educ. Res. 58, 61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.12.002

McNamara, D. S., O’Reilly, T. P., Best, R. M., and Ozuru, Y. (2006). Improving adolescent students’ reading comprehension with iSTART. Intern. J. Educ. Res. 34, 147–171. doi: 10.2190/1ru5-hdtj-a5c8-jvwe

Meyer, B. J., Wijekumar, K. K., and Lin, Y. C. (2011). Individualizing a web-based structure strategy intervention for fifth graders’ comprehension of nonfiction. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 140–168. doi: 10.1037/a0021606

Mich, O., Pianta, E., and Mana, N. (2013). Interactive stories and exercises with dynamic feedback for improving reading comprehension skills in deaf children. Comput. Educ. 65, 34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.01.016

Moran, J., Ferdig, R. E., Pearson, P. D., Wardrop, J., and Blomeyer, R. L. Jr. (2008). Technology and reading performance in the middle-school grades: a meta-analysis with recommendations for policy and practice. J. Liter. Res. 40, 6–58. doi: 10.1080/10862960802070483

Niedo, J., Lee, Y. L., Breznitz, Z., and Berninger, V. W. (2014). Computerized silent reading rate and strategy instruction for fourth graders at risk in silent reading rate. Learn. Disabil. Q. 37, 100–110. doi: 10.1177/0731948713507263

Oakhill, J. V., and Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Sci. Stud. Read. 162, 91–121. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2010.529219

Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., and Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: evidence from component skills. Lang. Cogn. Process. 18, 443–468. doi: 10.1080/01690960344000008

O’Reilly, T. P., Sinclair, G. P., and McNamara, D. S. (2004). “iSTART: A web-based reading strategy intervention that improves students’ science comprehension,” in Proceedings of the IADIS International Conference Cognition and Exploratory Learning in Digital Age: CELDA 2004 , eds D. G. Kinshuk and P. Isaias (Lisbon: IADIS), 173–180.

Ortlieb, E., Sargent, S., and Moreland, M. (2014). Evaluating the efficacy of using a digital reading environment to improve reading comprehension within a reading clinic. Read. Psychol. 35, 397–421. doi: 10.1080/02702711.2012.683236

Potocki, A., Ecalle, J., and Magnan, A. (2013). Effects of computer-assisted comprehension training in less skilled comprehenders in second grade: a one-year follow-up study. Comput. Educ. 63, 131–140. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.011

Potocki, A., Magnan, A., and Ecalle, J. (2015). Computerized trainings in four groups of struggling readers: specific effects on word reading and comprehension. Res. Dev. Disabil. 45, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.07.016

Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., and Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation M 2: Media in the Lives of 8-to 18-Year-Olds. San Francisco, CA: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.

Singer, L. M., and Alexander, P. A. (2017). Reading on paper and digitally: what the past decades of empirical research reveal. Rev. Educ. Res. 87, 1007–1041. doi: 10.3102/0034654317722961

Viola, F., and Carretti, B. (2019). Cambiamentinelleabilità di letturanelcorso di unostesso anno scolastico [Changes in readingskillsduring the sameschoolyear]. Dislessia 16, 147–159. doi: 10.14605/DIS1621902

von Bastian, C. C., and Oberauer, K. (2014). Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: a review. Psychol. Res. 78, 803–820. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0524-526

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wijekumar, K. K., Meyer, B. J., and Lei, P. (2012). Large-scale randomized controlled trial with 4th graders using intelligent tutoring of the structure strategy to improve nonfiction reading comprehension. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 60, 987–1013. doi: 10.1007/s11423-012-9263-4

Wijekumar, K. K., Meyer, B. J., and Lei, P. (2013). High-fidelity implementation of web-based intelligent tutoring system improves fourth and fifth graders content area reading comprehension. Comput. Educ. 68, 366–379. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.05.021

Wijekumar, K. K., Meyer, B. J., and Lei, P. (2017). Web-based text structure strategy instruction improves seventh graders’ content area reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 109, 741–760. doi: 10.1037/edu0000168

Woolley, G. (2011). “Reading comprehension,” in Reading Comprehension: Assisting Children With Learning Difficulties , ed Springer Science+Business Media (Dordrecht, NL: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-1174-7_2

Yuill, N., and Oakhill, J. (1988). Effects of inference awareness training on poor reading comprehension. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2, 33–45. doi: 10.1002/acp.2350020105

Keywords : reading comprehension, training, distance rehabilitation program, digital device, Cloze app

Citation: Capodieci A, Cornoldi C, Doerr E, Bertolo L and Carretti B (2020) The Use of New Technologies for Improving Reading Comprehension. Front. Psychol. 11:751. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00751

Received: 20 November 2019; Accepted: 27 March 2020; Published: 23 April 2020.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2020 Capodieci, Cornoldi, Doerr, Bertolo and Carretti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Agnese Capodieci, [email protected] ; Laura Bertolo, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Levels of Reading Comprehension in Higher Education: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Cristina de-la-peña.

1 Departamento de Métodos de Investigación y Diagnóstico en Educación, Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, Logroño, Spain

María Jesús Luque-Rojas

2 Department of Theory and History of Education and Research Methods and Diagnosis in Education, University of Malaga, Málaga, Spain

Associated Data

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Higher education aims for university students to produce knowledge from the critical reflection of scientific texts. Therefore, it is necessary to develop a deep mental representation of written information. The objective of this research was to determine through a systematic review and meta-analysis the proportion of university students who have an optimal performance at each level of reading comprehension. Systematic review of empirical studies has been limited from 2010 to March 2021 using the Web of Science, Scopus, Medline, and PsycINFO databases. Two reviewers performed data extraction independently. A random-effects model of proportions was used for the meta-analysis and heterogeneity was assessed with I 2 . To analyze the influence of moderating variables, meta-regression was used and two ways were used to study publication bias. Seven articles were identified with a total sample of the seven of 1,044. The proportion of students at the literal level was 56% (95% CI = 39–72%, I 2 = 96.3%), inferential level 33% (95% CI = 19–46%, I 2 = 95.2%), critical level 22% (95% CI = 9–35%, I 2 = 99.04%), and organizational level 22% (95% CI = 6–37%, I 2 = 99.67%). Comparing reading comprehension levels, there is a significant higher proportion of university students who have an optimal level of literal compared to the rest of the reading comprehension levels. The results have to be interpreted with caution but are a guide for future research.

Introduction

Reading comprehension allows the integration of knowledge that facilitates training processes and successful coping with academic and personal situations. In higher education, this reading comprehension has to provide students with autonomy to self-direct their academic-professional learning and provide critical thinking in favor of community service ( UNESCO, 2009 ). However, research in recent years ( Bharuthram, 2012 ; Afflerbach et al., 2015 ) indicates that a part of university students are not prepared to successfully deal with academic texts or they have reading difficulties ( Smagorinsky, 2001 ; Cox et al., 2014 ), which may limit academic training focused on written texts. This work aims to review the level of reading comprehension provided by studies carried out in different countries, considering the heterogeneity of existing educational models.

The level of reading comprehension refers to the type of mental representation that is made of the written text. The reader builds a mental model in which he can integrate explicit and implicit data from the text, experiences, and previous knowledge ( Kucer, 2016 ; van den Broek et al., 2016 ). Within the framework of the construction-integration model ( Kintsch and van Dijk, 1978 ; Kintsch, 1998 ), the most accepted model of reading comprehension, processing levels are differentiated, specifically: A superficial level that identifies or memorizes data forming the basis of the text and a deep level in which the text situation model is elaborated integrating previous experiences and knowledge. At these levels of processing, the cognitive strategies used, are different according to the domain-learning model ( Alexander, 2004 ) from basic coding to a transformation of the text. In the scientific literature, there are investigations ( Yussof et al., 2013 ; Ulum, 2016 ) that also identify levels of reading comprehension ranging from a literal level of identification of ideas to an inferential and critical level that require the elaboration of inferences and the data transformation.

Studies focused on higher education ( Barletta et al., 2005 ; Yáñez Botello, 2013 ) show that university students are at a literal or basic level of understanding, they often have difficulties in making inferences and recognizing the macrostructure of the written text, so they would not develop a model of a situation of the text. These scientific results are in the same direction as the research on reading comprehension in the mother tongue in the university population. Bharuthram (2012) indicates that university students do not access or develop effective strategies for reading comprehension, such as the capacity for abstraction and synthesis-analysis. Later, Livingston et al. (2015) find that first-year education students present limited reading strategies and difficulties in understanding written texts. Ntereke and Ramoroka (2017) found that only 12.4% of students perform well in a reading comprehension task, 34.3% presenting a low level of execution in the task.

Factors related to the level of understanding of written information are the mode of presentation of the text (printed vs. digital), the type of metacognitive strategies used (planning, making inferences, inhibition, monitoring, etc.), the type of text and difficulties (novel vs. a science passage), the mode of writing (text vs. multimodal), the type of reading comprehension task, and the diversity of the student. For example, several studies ( Tuncer and Bahadir, 2014 ; Trakhman et al., 2019 ; Kazazoglu, 2020 ) indicate that reading is more efficient with better performance in reading comprehension tests in printed texts compared to the same text in digital and according to Spencer (2006) college students prefer to read in print vs. digital texts. In reading the written text, metacognitive strategies are involved ( Amril et al., 2019 ) but studies ( Channa et al., 2018 ) seem to indicate that students do not use them for reading comprehension, specifically; Korotaeva (2012) finds that only 7% of students use them. Concerning the type of text and difficulties, for Wolfe and Woodwyk (2010) , expository texts benefit more from the construction of a situational model of the text than narrative texts, although Feng (2011) finds that expository texts are more difficult to read than narrative texts. Regarding the modality of the text, Mayer (2009) and Guo et al. (2020) indicate that multimodal texts that incorporate images into the text positively improve reading comprehension. In a study of Kobayashi (2002) using open questions, close, and multiple-choice shows that the type and format of the reading comprehension assessment test significantly influence student performance and that more structured tests help to better differentiate the good ones and the poor ones in reading comprehension. Finally, about student diversity, studies link reading comprehension with the interest and intrinsic motivation of university students ( Cartwright et al., 2019 ; Dewi et al., 2020 ), with gender ( Saracaloglu and Karasakaloglu, 2011 ), finding that women present a better level of reading comprehension than men and with knowledge related to reading ( Perfetti et al., 1987 ). In this research, it was controlled that all were printed and unimodal texts, that is, only text. This is essential because the cognitive processes involved in reading comprehension can vary with these factors ( Butcher and Kintsch, 2003 ; Xu et al., 2020 ).

The Present Study

Regardless of the educational context, in any university discipline, preparing essays or developing arguments are formative tasks that require a deep level of reading comprehension (inferences and transformation of information) that allows the elaboration of a situation model, and not having this level can lead to limited formative learning. Therefore, the objective of this research was to know the state of reading comprehension levels in higher education; specifically, the proportion of university students who perform optimally at each level of reading comprehension. It is important to note that there is not much information about the different levels in university students and that it is the only meta-analytic review that explores different levels of reading comprehension in this educational stage. This is a relevant issue because the university system requires that students produce knowledge from the critical reflection of scientific texts, preparing them for innovation, employability, and coexistence in society.

Materials and Methods

Eligibility criteria: inclusion and exclusion.

Empirical studies written in Spanish or English are selected that analyze the reading comprehension level in university students.

The exclusion criteria are as follows: (a) book chapters or review books or publications; (b) articles in other languages; (c) studies of lower educational levels; (d) articles that do not identify the age of the sample; (e) second language studies; (f) students with learning difficulties or other disorders; (g) publications that do not indicate the level of reading comprehension; (h) studies that relate reading competence with other variables but do not report reading comprehension levels; (i) pre-post program application work; (j) studies with experimental and control groups; (k) articles comparing pre-university stages or adults; (l) publications that use multi-texts; (m) studies that use some type of technology (computer, hypertext, web, psychophysiological, online questionnaire, etc.); and (n) studies unrelated to the subject of interest.

Only those publications that meet the following criteria are included as: (a) be empirical research (article, thesis, final degree/master’s degree, or conference proceedings book); (b) university stage; (c) include data or some measure on the level of reading comprehension that allows calculating the effect size; (d) written in English or Spanish; (e) reading comprehension in the first language or mother tongue; and (f) the temporary period from January 2010 to March 2021.

Search Strategies

A three-step procedure is used to select the studies included in the meta-analysis. In the first step, a review of research and empirical articles in English and Spanish from January 2010 to March 2021. The search is carried out in online databases of languages in Spanish and English, such as Web of Science (WoS), Scopus, Medline, and PsycINFO, to review empirical productions that analyze the level of reading comprehension in university students. In the second step, the following terms (titles, abstracts, keywords, and full text) are used to select the articles: Reading comprehension and higher education, university students, in Spanish and English, combined with the Boolean operators AND and OR. In the last step, secondary sources, such as the Google search engine, Theseus, and references in publications, are explored.

The search reports 4,294 publications (articles, theses, and conference proceedings books) in the databases and eight records of secondary references, specifically, 1989 from WoS, 2001 from Scopus, 42 from Medline, and 262 of PsycINFO. Of the total (4,294), 1,568 are eliminated due to duplications, leaving 2,734 valid records. Next, titles and abstracts are reviewed and 2,659 are excluded because they do not meet the inclusion criteria. The sample of 75 publications is reduced to 40 articles, excluding 35 because the full text cannot be accessed (the authors were contacted but did not respond), the full text did not show specific statistical data, they used online questionnaires or computerized presentations of the text. Finally, seven articles in Spanish were selected for use in the meta-analysis of the reading comprehension level of university students. Data additional to those included in the articles were not requested from the selected authors.

The PRISMA-P guidelines ( Moher et al., 2015 ) are followed to perform the meta-analysis and the flow chart for the selection of publications relevant to the subject is exposed (Figure 1) .

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-712901-g001.jpg

Flow diagram for the selection of articles.

Encoding Procedure

This research complies with what is established in the manual of systematic reviews ( Higgins and Green, 2008 ) in which clear objectives, specific search terms, and eligibility criteria for previously defined works are established. Two independent coders, reaching a 100% agreement, carry out the study search process. Subsequently, the research is codified, for this, a coding protocol is used as a guide to help resolve the ambiguities between the coders; the proposals are reflected and discussed and discrepancies are resolved, reaching a degree of agreement between the two coders of 97%.

For all studies, the reference, country, research objective, sample size, age and gender, reading comprehension test, other tests, and reading comprehension results were coded in percentages. All this information was later systematized in Table 1 .

Results of the empirical studies included in the meta-analysis.

In relation to the type of reading comprehension level, it was coded based on the levels of the scientific literature as follows: 1 = literal; 2 = inferential; 3 = critical; and 4 = organizational.

Regarding the possible moderating variables, it was coded if the investigations used a standardized reading comprehension measure (value = 1) or non-standardized (value = 0). This research considers the standardized measures of reading comprehension as the non-standardized measures created by the researchers themselves in their studies or questionnaires by other authors. By the type of evaluation test, we encode between multiple-choice (value = 0) or multiple-choices plus open question (value = 1). By type of text, we encode between argumentative (value = 1) or unknown (value = 0). By the type of career, we encode social sciences (value = 1) or other careers (health sciences; value = 0). Moreover, by the type of publication, we encode between article (value = 1) or doctoral thesis (value = 0).

Effect Size and Statistical Analysis

This descriptive study with a sample k = 7 and a population of 1,044 university students used a continuous variable and the proportions were used as the effect size to analyze the proportion of students who had an optimal performance at each level of reading comprehension. As for the percentages of each level of reading comprehension of the sample, they were transformed into absolute frequencies. A random-effects model ( Borenstein et al., 2009 ) was used as the effect size. These random-effects models have a greater capacity to generalize the conclusions and allow estimating the effects of different sources of variation (moderating variables). The DerSimonian and Laird method ( Egger et al., 2001 ) was used, calculating raw proportion and for each proportion its standard error, value of p and 95% confidence interval (CI).

To examine sampling variability, Cochran’s Q test (to test the null hypothesis of homogeneity between studies) and I 2 (proportion of variability) were used. According to Higgins et al. (2003) , if I 2 reaches 25%, it is considered low, if it reaches 50% and if it exceeds 75% it is considered high. A meta-regression analysis was used to investigate the effect of the moderator variables (type of measure, type of evaluation test, type of text, type of career, and type of publication) in each level of reading comprehension of the sample studies. For each moderating variable, all the necessary statistics were calculated (estimate, standard error, CI, Q , and I 2 ).

To compare the effect sizes of each level (literal, inferential, critical, and organizational) of reading comprehension, the chi-square test for the proportion recommended by Campbell (2007) was used.

Finally, to analyze publication bias, this study uses two ways: Rosenthal’s fail-safe number and regression test. Rosenthal’s fail-safe number shows the number of missing studies with null effects that would make the previous correlations insignificant ( Borenstein et al., 2009 ). When the values are large there is no bias. In the regression test, when the regression is not significant, there is no bias.

The software used to classify and encode data and produce descriptive statistics was with Microsoft Excel and the Jamovi version 1.6 free software was used to perform the meta-analysis.

The results of the meta-analysis are presented in three parts: the general descriptive analysis of the included studies; the meta-analytic analysis with the effect size, heterogeneity, moderating variables, and comparison of effect sizes; and the study of publication bias.

Overview of Included Studies

The search carried out of the scientific literature related to the subject published from 2010 to March 2021 generated a small number of publications, because it was limited to the higher education stage and required clear statistical data on reading comprehension.

Table 1 presents all the publications reviewed in this meta-analysis with a total of students evaluated in the reviewed works that amounts to 1,044, with the smallest sample size of 30 ( Del Pino-Yépez et al., 2019 ) and the largest with 570 ( Guevara Benítez et al., 2014 ). Regarding gender, 72% women and 28% men were included. Most of the sample comes from university degrees in social sciences, such as psychology and education (71.42%) followed by health sciences (14.28%) engineering and a publication (14.28%) that does not indicate origin. These publications selected according to the inclusion criteria for the meta-analysis come from more countries with a variety of educational systems, but all from South America. Specifically, the countries that have more studies are Mexico (28.57%) and Colombia, Chile, Bolivia, Peru, and Ecuador with 14.28% each, respectively. The years in which they were published are 2.57% in 2018 and 2016 and 14.28% in 2019, 2014, and 2013.

A total of 57% of the studies analyze four levels of reading comprehension (literal, inferential, critical, and organizational) and 43% investigate three levels of reading comprehension (literal, inferential, and critical). Based on the moderating variables, 57% of the studies use standardized reading comprehension measures and 43% non-standardized measures. According to the evaluation test used, 29% use multiple-choice questions and 71% combine multiple-choice questions plus open questions. 43% use an argumentative text and 57% other types of texts (not indicated in studies). By type of career, 71% are students of social sciences and 29% of other different careers, such as engineering or health sciences. In addition, 71% are articles and 29% with research works (thesis and degree works).

Table 2 shows the reading comprehension assessment instruments used by the authors of the empirical research integrated into the meta-analysis.

Reading comprehension assessment tests used in higher education.

Meta-Analytic Analysis of the Level of Reading Comprehension

The literal level presents a mean proportion effect size of 56% (95% CI = 39–72%; Figure 2 ). The variability between the different samples of the literal level of reading comprehension was significant ( Q = 162.066, p < 0.001; I 2 = 96.3%). No moderating variable used in this research had a significant contribution to heterogeneity: type of measurement ( p = 0.520), type of test ( p = 0.114), type of text ( p = 0.520), type of career ( p = 0.235), and type of publication ( p = 0.585). The high variability is explained by other factors not considered in this work, such as the characteristics of the students (cognitive abilities) or other issues.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-712901-g002.jpg

Forest plot of literal level.

The inferential level presents a mean proportion effect size of 33% (95% CI = 19–46%; Figure 3 ). The variability between the different samples of the inferential level of reading comprehension was significant ( Q = 125.123, p < 0.001; I 2 = 95.2%). The type of measure ( p = 0.011) and the type of text ( p = 0.011) had a significant contribution to heterogeneity. The rest of the variables had no significance: type of test ( p = 0.214), type of career ( p = 0.449), and type of publication ( p = 0.218). According to the type of measure, the proportion of students who have an optimal level in inferential administering a standardized test is 28.7% less than when a non-standardized test is administered. The type of measure reduces variability by 2.57% and explains the differences between the results of the studies at the inferential level. According to the type of text, the proportion of students who have an optimal level in inferential using an argumentative text is 28.7% less than when using another type of text. The type of text reduces the variability by 2.57% and explains the differences between the results of the studies at the inferential level.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-712901-g003.jpg

Forest plot of inferential level.

The critical level has a mean effect size of the proportion of 22% (95% CI = 9–35%; Figure 4 ). The variability between the different samples of the critical level of reading comprehension was significant ( Q = 627.044, p < 0.001; I 2 = 99.04%). No moderating variable used in this research had a significant contribution to heterogeneity: type of measurement ( p = 0.575), type of test ( p = 0.691), type of text ( p = 0.575), type of career ( p = 0.699), and type of publication ( p = 0.293). The high variability is explained by other factors not considered in this work, such as the characteristics of the students (cognitive abilities).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-712901-g004.jpg

Forest plot of critical level.

The organizational level presents a mean effect size of the proportion of 22% (95% CI = 6–37%; Figure 5 ). The variability between the different samples of the organizational level of reading comprehension was significant ( Q = 1799.366, p < 0.001; I 2 = 99.67%). The type of test made a significant contribution to heterogeneity ( p = 0.289). The other moderating variables were not significant in this research: type of measurement ( p = 0.289), type of text ( p = 0.289), type of career ( p = 0.361), and type of publication ( p = 0.371). Depending on the type of test, the proportion of students who have an optimal level in organizational with multiple-choices tests plus open questions is 37% higher than while using only multiple-choice tests. The type of text reduces the variability by 0.27% and explains the differences between the results of the studies at the organizational level.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-12-712901-g005.jpg

Forest plot of organizational level.

Table 3 shows the difference between the estimated effect sizes and the significance. There is a larger proportion of students having an optimal level of reading comprehension at the literal level compared to the inferential, critical, and organizational level; an optimal level of reading comprehension at the inferential level vs. the critical and organizational level.

Results of effect size comparison.

Analysis of Publication Bias

This research uses two ways to verify the existence of bias independently of the sample size. Table 4 shows the results and there is no publication bias at any level of reading comprehension.

Publication bias results.

This research used a systematic literature search and meta-analysis to provide estimates of the number of cases of university students who have an optimal level in the different levels of reading comprehension. All the information available on the subject at the international level was analyzed using international databases in English and Spanish, but the potentially relevant publications were limited. Only seven Spanish language studies were identified internationally. In these seven studies, the optimal performance at each level of reading comprehension varied, finding heterogeneity associated with the very high estimates, which indicates that the summary estimates have to be interpreted with caution and in the context of the sample and the variables used in this meta-analysis.

In this research, the effects of the type of measure, type of test, type of text, type of career, and type of publication have been analyzed. Due to the limited information in the publications, it was not possible to assess the effect of any more moderating variables.

We found that some factors significantly influence heterogeneity according to the level of reading comprehension considered. The type of measure influenced the optimal performance of students in the inferential level of reading comprehension; specifically, the proportion of students who have an optimal level in inferential worsens if the test is standardized. Several studies ( Pike, 1996 ; Koretz, 2002 ) identify differences between standardized and non-standardized measures in reading comprehension and a favor of non-standardized measures developed by the researchers ( Pyle et al., 2017 ). The ability to generate inferences of each individual may difficult to standardize because each person differently identifies the relationship between the parts of the text and integrates it with their previous knowledge ( Oakhill, 1982 ; Cain et al., 2004 ). This mental representation of the meaning of the text is necessary to create a model of the situation and a deep understanding ( McNamara and Magliano, 2009 ; van den Broek and Espin, 2012 ).

The type of test was significant for the organizational level of reading comprehension. The proportion of students who have an optimal level in organizational improves if the reading comprehension assessment test is multiple-choice plus open questions. The organizational level requires the reordering of written information through analysis and synthesis processes ( Guevara Benítez et al., 2014 ); therefore, it constitutes a production task that is better reflected in open questions than in reproduction questions as multiple choice ( Dinsmore and Alexander, 2015 ). McNamara and Kintsch (1996) identify that open tasks require an effort to make inferences related to previous knowledge and multidisciplinary knowledge. Important is to indicate that different evaluation test formats can measure different aspects of reading comprehension ( Zheng et al., 2007 ).

The type of text significantly influenced the inferential level of reading comprehension. The proportion of students who have an optimal level in inferential decreases with an argumentative text. The expectations created before an argumentative text made it difficult to generate inferences and, therefore, the construction of the meaning of the text. This result is in the opposite direction to the study by Diakidoy et al. (2011) who find that the refutation text, such as the argumentative one, facilitates the elaboration of inferences compared to other types of texts. It is possible that the argumentative text, given its dialogical nature of arguments and counterarguments, with a subject unknown by the students, has determined the decrease of inferences based on their scarce previous knowledge of the subject, needing help to elaborate the structure of the text read ( Reznitskaya et al., 2007 ). It should be pointed out that in meta-analysis studies, 43% use argumentative texts. Knowing the type of the text is relevant for generating inferences, for instance, according to Baretta et al. (2009) the different types of text are processed differently in the brain generating more or fewer inferences; specifically, using the N400 component, they find that expository texts generate more inferences from the text read.

For the type of career and the type of publication, no significance was found at any level of reading comprehension in this sample. This seems to indicate that university students have the same level of performance in tasks of literal, critical inferential, and organizational understanding regardless of whether they are studying social sciences, health sciences, or engineering. Nor does the type of publication affect the state of the different levels of reading comprehension in higher education.

The remaining high heterogeneity at all levels of reading comprehension was not captured in this review, indicating that there are other factors, such as student characteristics, gender, or other issues, that are moderating and explaining the variability at the literal, inferential, critical, and organizational reading comprehension in university students.

To the comparison between the different levels of reading comprehension, the literal level has a significantly higher proportion of students with an optimal level than the inferential, critical, and organizational levels. The inferential level has a significantly higher proportion of students with an optimal level than the critical and organizational levels. This corresponds with data from other investigations ( Márquez et al., 2016 ; Del Pino-Yépez et al., 2019 ) that indicate that the literal level is where university students execute with more successes, being more difficult and with less success at the inferential, organizational, and critical levels. This indicates that university students of this sample do not generate a coherent situation model that provides them with a global mental representation of the read text according to the model of Kintsch (1998) , but rather they make a literal analysis of the explicit content of the read text. This level of understanding can lead to less desirable results in educational terms ( Dinsmore and Alexander, 2015 ).

The educational implications of this meta-analysis in this sample are aimed at making universities aware of the state of reading comprehension levels possessed by university students and designing strategies (courses and workshops) to optimize it by improving the training and employability of students. Some proposals can be directed to the use of reflection tasks, integration of information, graphic organizers, evaluation, interpretation, nor the use of paraphrasing ( Rahmani, 2011 ). Some studies ( Hong-Nam and Leavell, 2011 ; Parr and Woloshyn, 2013 ) demonstrate the effectiveness of instructional courses in improving performance in reading comprehension and metacognitive strategies. In addition, it is necessary to design reading comprehension assessment tests in higher education that are balanced, validated, and reliable, allowing to have data for the different levels of reading comprehension.

Limitations and Conclusion

This meta-analysis can be used as a starting point to report on reading comprehension levels in higher education, but the results should be interpreted with caution and in the context of the study sample and variables. Publications without sufficient data and inaccessible articles, with a sample of seven studies, may have limited the international perspective. The interest in studying reading comprehension in the mother tongue, using only unimodal texts, without the influence of technology and with English and Spanish has also limited the review. The limited amount of data in the studies has limited meta-regression.

This review is a guide to direct future research, broadening the study focus on the level of reading comprehension using digital technology, experimental designs, second languages, and investigations that relate reading comprehension with other factors (gender, cognitive abilities, etc.) that can explain the heterogeneity in the different levels of reading comprehension. The possibility of developing a comprehensive reading comprehension assessment test in higher education could also be explored.

This review contributes to the scientific literature in several ways. In the first place, this meta-analytic review is the only one that analyzes the proportion of university students who have an optimal performance in the different levels of reading comprehension. This review is made with international publications and this topic is mostly investigated in Latin America. Second, optimal performance can be improved at all levels of reading comprehension, fundamentally inferential, critical, and organizational. The literal level is significantly the level of reading comprehension with the highest proportion of optimal performance in university students. Third, the students in this sample have optimal performance at the inferential level when they are non-argumentative texts and non-standardized measures, and, in the analyzed works, there is optimal performance at the organizational level when multiple-choice questions plus open questions are used.

The current research is linked to the research project “Study of reading comprehension in higher education” of Asociación Educar para el Desarrollo Humano from Argentina.

Data Availability Statement

Author contributions.

Cd-l-P had the idea for the article and analyzed the data. ML-R searched the data. Cd-l-P and ML-R selected the data and contributed to the valuable comments and manuscript writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The handling editor declared a shared affiliation though no other collaboration with one of the authors ML-R at the time of the review.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Funding. This paper was funded by the Universidad Internacional de la Rioja and Universidad de Málaga.

  • Afflerbach P., Cho B.-Y., Kim J.-Y. (2015). Conceptualizing and assessing higher-order thinking in reading . Theory Pract. 54 , 203–212. 10.1080/00405841.2015.1044367 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Alexander P. A. (2004). “ A model of domain learning: reinterpreting expertise as a multidimensional, multistage process ,” in Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition: Integrative Perspectives on Intellectual Functioning and Development. eds. Dai D. Y., Sternberg R. J. (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; ), 273–298. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Amril A., Hasanuddin W. S., Atmazaki (2019). The contributions of reading strategies and reading frequencies toward students’ reading comprehension skill in higher education . Int. J. Eng. Adv. Technol. (IJEAT) 8 , 593–595. 10.35940/ijeat.F1105.0986S319 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baretta L., Braga Tomitch L. M., MacNair N., Kwan Lim V., Waldie K. E. (2009). Inference making while reading narrative and expository texts: an ERP study . Psychol. Neurosci. 2 , 137–145. 10.3922/j.psns.2009.2.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barletta M., Bovea V., Delgado P., Del Villar L., Lozano A., May O., et al.. (2005). Comprensión y Competencias Lectoras en Estudiantes Universitarios. Barranquilla: Uninorte. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bharuthram S. (2012). Making a case for the teaching of reading across the curriculum in higher education . S. Afr. J. Educ. 32 , 205–214. 10.15700/saje.v32n2a557 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., Rothstein H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. United Kingdom: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd, 45–49. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butcher K. R., Kintsch W. (2003). “ Text comprehension and discourse processing ,” in Handbook of Psychology: Experimental Psychology. 2nd Edn . Vol . 4 . eds. Healy A. F., Proctor R. W., Weiner I. B. (New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.), 575–595. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cain K., Oakhill J., Bryant P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills . J. Educ. Psychol. 96 , 31–42. 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell I. (2007). Chi-squared and Fisher-Irwin tests of two-by-two tables with small sample recommendations . Stat. Med. 26 , 3661–3675. 10.1002/sim.2832, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cartwright K. B., Lee S. A., Barber A. T., DeWyngaert L. U., Lane A. B., Singleton T. (2019). Contributions of executive function and cognitive intrinsic motivation to university students’ reading comprehension . Read. Res. Q. 55 , 345–369. 10.1002/rrq.273 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Channa M. A., Abassi A. M., John S., Sahito J. K. M. (2018). Reading comprehension and metacognitive strategies in first-year engineering university students in Pakistan . Int. J. Engl. Ling. 8 , 78–87. 10.5539/ijel.v8n6p78 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cox S. R., Friesner D. L., Khayum M. (2014). Do Reading skills courses help underprepared readers achieve academic success in college? J. College Reading and Learn. 33 , 170–196. 10.1080/10790195.2003.10850147 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Del Pino-Yépez G. M., Saltos-Rodríguez L. J., Moreira-Aguayo P. Y. (2019). Estrategias didácticas para el afianzamiento de la comprensión lectora en estudiantes universitarios . Revista científica Dominio de las Ciencias 5 , 171–187. 10.23857/dc.v5i1.1038 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dewi R. S., Fahrurrozi, Hasanah U., Wahyudi A. (2020). Reading interest and Reading comprehension: a correlational study in Syarif Hidayatullah State Islamic University, Jakarta . Talent Dev. Excell. 12 , 241–250. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diakidoy I. N., Mouskounti T., Ioannides C. (2011). Comprehension and learning from refutation and expository texts . Read. Res. Q. 46 , 22–38. 10.1598/RRQ.46.1.2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dinsmore D. J., Alexander P. A. (2015). A multidimensional investigation of deep-level and surface-level processing . J. Exp. Educ. 84 , 213–244. 10.1080/00220973.2014.979126 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Egger M., Smith D., Altmand D. G. (2001). Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-Analysis in Context. London: BMJ Publishing Group. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feng L. (2011). A short analysis of the text variables affecting reading and testing reading . Stud. Lit. Lang. 2 , 44–49. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Figueroa Romero R. L., Castañeda Sánchez W., Tamay Carranza I. A. (2016). Nivel de comprensión lectora en los estudiantes del primer ciclo de la Universidad San Pedro, filial Caraz, 2016. (Trabajo de investigación, Universidad San Pedro). Repositorio Institucional USP. Available at: http://repositorio.usanpedro.edu.pe/bitstream/handle/USANPEDRO/305/PI1640418.pdf?sequence=1andisAllowed=y (Accessed February 15, 2021).
  • Guevara Benítez Y., Guerra García J., Delgado Sánchez U., Flores Rubí C. (2014). Evaluación de distintos niveles de comprensión lectora en estudiantes mexicanos de Psicología . Acta Colombiana de Psicología 17 , 113–121. 10.14718/ACP.2014.17.2.12 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guo D., Zhang S., Wright K. L., McTigue E. M. (2020). Do you get the picture? A meta-analysis of the effect of graphics on reading comprehension . AERA Open 6 , 1–20. 10.1177/2332858420901696 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins J. P., Green S. (2008). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. The Cochrane Collaboration. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Higgins J. P., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses . BMJ 327 , 327–557. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong-Nam K., Leavell A. G. (2011). Reading strategy instruction, metacognitive awareness, and self-perception of striving college developmental readers . J. College Literacy Learn. 37 , 3–17. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kazazoglu S. (2020). Is printed text the best choice? A mixed-method case study on reading comprehension . J. Lang. Linguistic Stud. 16 , 458–473. 10.17263/jlls.712879 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kintsch W. (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kintsch W., van Dijk T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production . Psychol. Rev. 85 , 363–394. 10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.363 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kobayashi M. (2002). Method effects on reading comprehension test performance: test organization and response format . Lang. Test. 19 , 193–220. 10.1191/0265532202lt227oa [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koretz D. (2002). Limitations in the use of achievement tests as measures of educators’ productivity . J. Hum. Resour. 37 , 752–777. 10.2307/3069616 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Korotaeva I. V. (2012). Metacognitive strategies in reading comprehension of education majors . Procedural-Social and Behav. Sci. 69 , 1895–1900. 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.143 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kucer S. B. (2016). Accuracy, miscues, and the comprehension of complex literary and scientific texts . Read. Psychol. 37 , 1076–1095. 10.1080/02702711.2016.1159632 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Livingston C., Klopper B., Cox S., Uys C. (2015). The impact of an academic reading program in the bachelor of education (intermediate and senior phase) degree . Read. Writ. 6 , 1–11. 10.4102/rw.v6i1.66 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Márquez H., Díaz C., Muñoz R., Fuentes R. (2016). Evaluación de los niveles de comprensión lectora en estudiantes universitarios pertenecientes a las carreras de Kinesiología y Nutrición y Dietética de la Universidad Andrés Bello, Concepción . Revista de Educación en Ciencias de la Salud 13 , 154–160. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer R. E. (ed.) (2009). “ Modality principle ,” in Multimedia Learning. United States: Cambridge University Press, 200–2020. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNamara D. S., Kintsch W. (1996). Learning from texts: effects of prior knowledge and text coherence . Discourse Process 22 , 247–288. 10.1080/01638539609544975 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNamara D. S., Magliano J. (2009). “ Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension ,” in The psychology of learning and motivation. ed. Ross E. B. (New York: Elsevier; ), 297–384. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moher D., Shamseer L., Clarke M., Ghersi D., Liberati A., Petticrew M., et al.. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement . Syst. Rev. 4 :1. 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ntereke B. B., Ramoroka B. T. (2017). Reading competency of first-year undergraduate students at the University of Botswana: a case study . Read. Writ. 8 :a123. 10.4102/rw.v8i1.123 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oakhill J. (1982). Constructive processes in skilled and less skilled comprehenders’ memory for sentences . Br. J. Psychol. 73 , 13–20. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1982.tb01785.x, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parr C., Woloshyn V. (2013). Reading comprehension strategy instruction in a first-year course: an instructor’s self-study . Can. J. Scholarship Teach. Learn. 4 :3. 10.5206/cjsotl-rcacea.2013.2.3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perfetti C. A., Beck I., Bell L. C., Hughes C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: a longitudinal study of first-grade children . Merrill-Palmer Q. 33 , 283–319. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pike G. R. (1996). Limitations of using students’ self-reports of academic development as proxies for traditional achievement measures . Res. High. Educ. 37 , 89–114. 10.1007/BF01680043 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pyle N., Vasquez A. C., Lignugaris B., Gillam S. L., Reutzel D. R., Olszewski A., et al.. (2017). Effects of expository text structure interventions on comprehension: a meta-analysis . Read. Res. Q. 52 , 469–501. 10.1002/rrq.179 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rahmani M. (2011). Effects of note-taking training on reading comprehension and recall . Reading Matrix: An Int. Online J. 11 , 116–126. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reznitskaya A., Anderson R., Kuo L. J. (2007). Teaching and learning argumentation . Elem. Sch. J. 107 , 449–472. 10.1086/518623 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sáez Sánchez B. K. (2018). La comprensión lectora en jóvenes universitarios de una escuela formadora de docentes . Revista Electrónica Científica de Investigación Educativa 4 , 609–618. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sanabria Mantilla T. R. (2018). Relación entre comprensión lectora y rendimiento académico en estudiantes de primer año de Psicología de la Universidad Pontificia Bolivariana. (Trabajo de grado, Universidad Pontificia Bolivariana). Repositorio Institucional UPB. Available at: https://repository.upb.edu.co/bitstream/handle/20.500.11912/5443/digital_36863.pdf?sequence=1andisAllowed=y (Accessed February 15, 2021).
  • Saracaloglu A. S., Karasakaloglu N. (2011). An investigation of prospective teachers’ reading comprehension levels and study and learning strategies related to some variables . Egit. ve Bilim 36 , 98–115. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smagorinsky P. (2001). If meaning is constructed, what is it made from? Toward a cultural theory of reading . Rev. Educ. Res. 71 , 133–169. 10.3102/00346543071001133 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spencer C. (2006). Research on learners’ preferences for reading from a printed text or a computer screen . J. Dist. Educ. 21 , 33–50. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trakhman L. M. S., Alexander P., Berkowitz L. E. (2019). Effects of processing time on comprehension and calibration in print and digital mediums . J. Exp. Educ. 87 , 101–115. 10.1080/00220973.2017.1411877 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tuncer M., Bhadir F. (2014). Effect of screen reading and reading from printed out material on student success and permanency in introduction to computer lesson . Turk. Online J. Educ. Technol. 13 , 41–49. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ulum O. G. (2016). A descriptive content analysis of the extent of Bloom’s taxonomy in the reading comprehension questions of the course book Q: skills for success 4 reading and writing . Qual. Rep. 21 , 1674–1683. [ Google Scholar ]
  • UNESCO (2009). “Conferencia mundial sobre la Educación Superior – 2009.” La nueva dinámica de la educación superior y la investigación para el cambio social y el desarrollo; July 5-8, 2009; Paris.
  • van den Broek P., Espin C. A. (2012). Connecting cognitive theory and assessment: measuring individual differences in reading comprehension . Sch. Psychol. Rev. 43 , 315–325. 10.1080/02796015.2012.12087512 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van den Broek P., Mouw J. M., Kraal A. (2016). “ Individual differences in reading comprehension ,” in Handbook of Individual Differences in Reading: Reader, Text, and Context. ed. Afflerbach E. P. (New York: Routledge; ), 138–150. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wolfe M. B. W., Woodwyk J. M. (2010). Processing and memory of information presented in narrative or expository texts . Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 80 , 341–362. 10.1348/000709910X485700, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xu Y., Wong R., He S., Veldre A., Andrews S. (2020). Is it smart to read on your phone? The impact of reading format and culture on the continued influence of misinformation . Mem. Cogn. 48 , 1112–1127. 10.3758/s13421-020-01046-0, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yáñez Botello C. R. (2013). Caracterización de los procesos cognoscitivos y competencias involucrados en los niveles de comprensión lectora en Estudiantes Universitarios . Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos de Psicología 13 , 75–90. 10.18270/chps.v13i2.1350 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yussof Y. M., Jamian A. R., Hamzah Z. A. Z., Roslan A. (2013). Students’ reading comprehension performance with emotional . Int. J. Edu. Literacy Stud. 1 , 82–88. 10.7575/aiac.ijels.v.1n.1p.82 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zheng Y., Cheng L., Klinger D. A. (2007). Do test format in reading comprehension affect second-language students’ test performance differently? TESL Can. J. 25 , 65–78. 10.18806/tesl.v25i1.108 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Log in to Witsby: ASCD’s Next-Generation Professional Learning and Credentialing Platform

Synthesis of Research / Reading Comprehension: What Works

Ample time for text reading, getting the most out of reading time, teacher-directed instruction, opportunities for peer and collaborative learning, time to talk about reading, a call for multiple approaches.

  • large amounts of time for actual text reading,
  • teacher-directed instruction in comprehension strategies,
  • opportunities for peer and collaborative learning, and
  • occasions for students to talk to a teacher and one another about their responses to reading.
  • Choice . Teachers can give children opportunities and guidance in making text selections. Although we know of no research that directly links choice to reading comprehension growth, we speculate that choice is related to interest and motivation, both of which are related directly to learning (Anderson et al. 1987).
  • Optimal difficulty . Teachers can monitor students' and their own selections to ensure that all students spend most of their time reading books that are appropriate in difficulty—not so hard that a student's cognitive resources are occupied with just figuring out how to pronounce the words and not so easy that nothing new is likely to be learned.
  • Multiple readings . Teachers can honor and encourage rereading of texts, which research suggests leads to greater fluency and comprehension (Allington 1983a). Although most research about repeated reading of passages has focused on improvements in reading speed, accuracy, phrasing, and intonation, a growing number of studies have documented improved comprehension as well (Dowhower 1987).
  • Negotiating meaning socially . “Silent” reading time shouldn't be entirely silent. Teachers can (a) allow part of the time for reading in pairs, including pairs of different abilities and ages (Koskinen and Blum 1986, Labbo and Teale 1990); and (b) provide regular opportunities for readers to discuss their reading with the teacher and with one another. We view reading comprehension as a social as well as a cognitive process. Conversation not only raises the status of independent silent reading from a time filler to an important part of the reading program; it also gives students another opportunity to practice and build comprehension skills collaboratively, a topic to which we return below. Atwell (1987) and Hansen (1987) further argue that these conversations help to build the all-important community of readers that is the essence of literature-based programs.
  • using background knowledge to make inferences (Hansen and Pearson 1983) or set purposes (Ogle 1986);
  • getting the main idea (Baumann 1984);
  • identifying the sources of information needed to answer a question (Raphael and Pearson 1985); and
  • using the typical structure of stories (Fitzgerald and Spiegel 1983) or expository texts (Armbruster et al. 1987) to help students understand what they are reading.
  • Changing teacher-student interaction patterns . In the traditional recitation format, teachers choose the topics and, through feedback to students, control which student answers are viewed as correct and incorrect. One outcome of the recitation format is that teachers talk a lot! Typically, teachers talk as much as or more than all students combined, because their questions and feedback focus on transmitting the text interpretation they have in mind and because of the monitoring function that teachers naturally perform when they are in charge of a discussion. Tharp and Gallimore (1989) use the terms responsive teaching and instructional conversations to contrast effective teacher-student dialogues with such recitations. In responsive teaching, teachers plan instruction by anticipating a range of student responses in addition to thinking about their own interpretations. They then use student input into discussions and student text interpretations to move the discussion to higher levels. Teachers might still nominate topics and opinions for group consideration, but student input drives the discussion forward. Changing the pattern of classroom discussions to allow more student input and control is no easy task. Alvermann and Hayes (1989), for example, found that it was much easier for teachers to change the level of questions they asked (for example, move to more inferential, evaluative, and critical thinking questions) than it was for them to change the basic structure or pattern of interactions in classroom discussions. Teachers suggested two main reasons for the persistence of the recitation format in their classrooms: maintaining control and ensuring coverage of important information and canonical interpretations.
  • [[[[[ **** LIST ITEM IGNORED **** ]]]]]
  • Embedding strategy instruction in text reading . Even in teacher-student discussions focused around a shared understanding of important text information, new ideas are emerging about how to build this shared understanding in a way that will teach students something about comprehension as well as text information. For example, in situated cognition (Brown et al. 1989), learning about comprehension strategies is embedded in discussions about texts. The cognitive activities students engage in are much like the ones that have been the focus of research about explicit instruction in comprehension strategies, such as summarizing and getting the main idea. The difference is that the focus is on learning authentic information in the texts—for example, discovering how photosynthesis works by reading a chapter about it—with comprehension strategy learning as a secondary outcome of repeated engagement in such discussions about many different texts. The belief is that students will internalize effective comprehension strategies through repeated situations in which they read and discuss whole texts with a teacher and peers.

Allington, R. L. (1983a). “Fluency: The Neglected Reading Goal.” The Reading Teacher 36: 556–561.

Allington, R. L. (1983b). “The Reading Instruction Provided Readers of Differing Reading Abilities.” Elementary School Journal 83: 548–559.

Alvermann, D. E., and D. A. Hayes. (1989). “Classroom Discussion of Content Area Reading Assignments: An Intervention Study.” Reading Research Quarterly 24: 305–335.

Anderson, R. C., E. H. Hiebert, J. A. Scott, and I. A. G. Wilkinson. (1985). Becoming a Nation of Readers . Washington, D. C.: National Institute of Education.

Anderson, R. C., L. Shirey, P. T. Wilson, and L. G. Fielding. (1987). “Interestingness of Children's Reading Material.” In Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction. Vol. 3: Conative and Affective Process Analyses , edited by R. Snow and M. Farr. Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

Anderson, R. C., P. T. Wilson, and L. G. Fielding. (1988). “Growth in Reading and How Children Spend Their Time Outside of School.” Reading Research Quarterly 23: 285–303.

Armbruster, B. B., T. H. Anderson, and J. Ostertag. (1987). “Does Text Structure/Summarization Instruction Facilitate Learning From Expository Text?” Reading Research Quarterly 22: 331–346.

Atwell, N. (1987). In the Middle . Montclair, N. J.: Boynton/Cook.

Baumann, J. F. (1984). “Effectiveness of a Direct Instruction Paradigm for Teaching Main Idea Comprehension.” Reading Research Quarterly 20: 93–108.

Beach, R., and S. Hynds. (1991). “Research on Response to Literature.” In Handbook of Reading Research: Vol. II , edited by R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson. New York: Longman.

Beck, I. L., R. C. Omanson, and M. G. McKeown. (1982). “An Instructional Redesign of Reading Lessons: Effects on Comprehension.” Reading Research Quarterly 17: 462–481.

Brown, J. S., A. Collins, and P. Duguid. (1989). “Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning.” Educational Researcher 18, 1: 32–42.

Cazden, C. (1986). “Classroom Discourse.” In Handbook of Research on Teaching , 3rd ed., edited by M. C. Wittrock. New York: Macmillan.

Delpit, L. (1988). “The Silenced Dialogue: Power and Pedagogy in Educating Other People's Children.” Harvard Educational Review 58, 3: 280–298.

Dowhower, S. L. (1987). “Effects of Repeated Reading on Second-Grade Transitional Readers' Fluency and Comprehension.” Reading Research Quarterly 22: 389–406.

Duffy, G., L. Roehler, and B. Hermann. (1988). “Modeling Mental Processes Helps Poor Readers Become Strategic Readers.” The Reading Teacher 41: 762–767.

Durkin, D. (1978–1979). “What Classroom Observations Reveal About Reading Comprehension Instruction.” Reading Research Quarterly 15: 481–533.

Eeds, M., and D. Wells. (1989). “Grand Conversations: An Exploration of Meaning Construction in Literature Study Groups.” Research in the Teaching of English 23: 4–29.

Elley, W. B. (1989). “Vocabulary Acquisition from Listening to Stories.” Reading Research Quarterly 24: 174–187.

Fitzgerald, J., and D. L. Spiegel. (1983). “Enhancing Children's Reading Comprehension Through Instruction in Narrative Structure.” Journal of Reading Behavior 15, 2: 1–17.

Hansen, J. (1987). When Writers Read . Portsmouth, N. H.: Heinemann.

Hansen, J., and P. D. Pearson. (1983). “An Instructional Study: Improving Inferential Comprehension of Good and Poor Fourth-Grade Readers.” Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 821–829.

Johnson, D., and R. Johnson. (1985). “The Internal Dynamics of Cooperative Learning Groups.” In Learning to Cooperate, Cooperating to Learn , edited by R. Slavin, S. Sharon, S. Kagan, R. Hertz-Lazarowitz, C. Webb, and R. Schmuck. New York: Plenum Press.

Koskinen, P., and I. Blum. (1986). “Paired Repeated Reading: A Classroom Strategy for Developing Fluent Reading.” The Reading Teacher 40: 70–75.

Labbo, L., and W. Teale. (1990). “Cross-Age Reading: A Strategy for Helping Poor Readers.” The Reading Teacher 43: 362–369.

Leinhardt, G., N. Zigmond, and W. Cooley. (1981). “Reading Instruction and Its Effects.” American Educational Research Journal 18: 343–361.

Manning, G. L., and M. Manning. (1984). “What Models of Recreational Reading Make a Difference?” Reading World 23: 375–380.

Meloth, M. (1991). “Enhancing Literacy Through Cooperative Learning.” In Literacy for a Diverse Society: Perspectives, Practices, and Policies , edited by E. Hiebert. New York: Teachers College Press.

Nagy, W. E., R. C. Anderson, and P. A. Herman. (1987). “Learning Word Meanings from Context During Normal Reading.” American Educational Research Journal 24: 237–270.

Ogle, D. (1986). “K-W-L: A Teaching Model That Develops Active Reading of Expository Text.” The Reading Teacher 39: 564–570.

Palincsar, A. S., A. L. Brown, and S. M. Martin. (1987). “Peer Interaction in Reading Comprehension Instruction.” Educational Psychologist 22: 231–253.

Paris, S. G., B. A. Wasik, and J. C. Turner. (1991). “The Development of Strategic Readers.” In Handbook of Reading Research: Vol. II , edited by R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson New York: Longman.

Pearson, P. D., and J. A. Dole. (1987). “Explicit Comprehension Instruction: A Review of Research and a New Conceptualization of Instruction.” Elementary School Journal 88, 2: 151–165.

Pearson, P. D., and L. G. Fielding. (1991). “Comprehension Instruction.” In Handbook of Reading Research: Vol. II , edited by R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson. New York: Longman.

Peterson, R., and M. Eeds. (1990). Grand Conversations: Literature Groups in Action . New York: Scholastic.

Raphael, T., S. McMahon, V. Goatley, J. Bentley, F. Boyd, L. Pardo, and D. Woodman. (1992). “Research Directions: Literature and Discussion in the Reading Program.” Language Arts 69: 54–61.

Raphael, T. E., and P. D. Pearson. (1985). “Increasing Students' Awareness of Sources of Information for Answering Questions.” American Educational Research Journal 22: 217–236.

Rogers, T. (1991). “Students as Literary Critics: The Interpretive Experiences, Beliefs, and Processes of Ninth-Grade Students.” Journal of Reading Behavior 23: 391–423.

Rosenblatt, L. (1978). The Reader, the Text, the Poem: The Transactional Theory of a Literary Work . Carbondale, Ill.: Southern Illinois University Press.

Slavin, R. E. (1987). “Cooperative Learning and the Cooperative School.” Educational Leadership 45, 3: 7–13.

Stallman, A. (1991). “Learning Vocabulary from Context: Effects of Focusing Attention on Individual Words During Reading.” Doctoral diss., University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.

Stanovich, K. (1986). “Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy.” Reading Research Quarterly 21: 360–407.

Stevens, R., N. Madden, R. Slavin, and A. Farnish. (1987). “Cooperative Integrated Reading and Composition: Two Field Experiments.” Reading Research Quarterly 22: 433–454.

Tharp, R. G., and R. Gallimore. (1989). Rousing Minds to Life: Teaching, Learning and Schooling in Social Context . New York: Cambridge University Press.

research topic in reading comprehension

Linda G. Fielding has been a contributor to Educational Leadership.

P. David Pearson has been a contributor to Educational Leadership.

ASCD is a community dedicated to educators' professional growth and well-being.

Let us help you put your vision into action., from our issue.

Product cover image 194025.jpg

To process a transaction with a Purchase Order please send to [email protected]

  • Our Mission

Slowing Down the Reading Process to Build Students’ Comprehension Skills

Slow reading gives students time to deeply engage with texts and gather their thoughts about the content.

Illustration of a book shelf shaped like a person's profile

When we read deeply, parts of our brain associated with analytical thought, empathy, and inference are activated. This happens whether we’re contemplating Ophelia’s motivations in Hamlet or analyzing what exactly lives inside the microbiome of the human body. Literature in particular does some pretty amazing things to our neural systems: It activates the parts of our brain associated with movement, texture, and affect. Even though we may be sitting on a train to Brooklyn, our brains process the experience of floating in a swimming pool as if we were actually living the experience of the protagonist in Starfish . Deep reading also enhances analytical skills. 

Critically reading nonfiction texts builds background knowledge that helps us to integrate information into the mental schema necessary for deeper learning. When reading nonfiction deeply, we ask ourselves, “Does this square with what I know? Can I make the author’s assumptions my own?”

By teaching students to build the habit of slowing down while they read, we can also help them reap the benefits of deep reading. 

Build Students’ Reading Stamina Over Time

What if the novel assigned in English class is the only book a student reads that year? Often, it is. In 2018, researcher Jean Twenge concluded that as students’ time on social media increased outside of school, they were graduating from high school with less practice focusing on long texts . Imagine the difficulty these students must have when a five-page read in high school or college demands their sustained, undivided attention. Many of the middle and high school teachers I work with see their students struggle to remain focused on longer, grade-level text daily.

Curbing students’ appetite for digital distractions is out of most teachers’ control, but a teacher still largely guides what happens in a single period. If we want to build students’ capability to critically read longer texts, they need intentional practice during the school day. It’s not just novels, either: the National Council of Teachers of English’s position statement on nonfiction literature calls for building young people’s reading stamina with nonfiction texts in particular. Any teacher who has trudged through a 300-page novel with seventh graders knows that this can take time. And by time, I mean months. Novels and longer pieces of nonfiction literature take time to read in their entirety, especially when the class is pausing frequently to integrate writing and discussion—which is important.

Use Graphic Organizers

Middle school students tend to be keenly aware of who the fast readers are in class. However, reading quickly isn’t the same as reading deeply. Information intake is improved by small, intentional outputs when students read. This purposeful production of output has the added result of slowing kids down in a way that takes their learning deeper rather than faster. 

In my early years of teaching, my students and I didn’t consider the “why” or the “which” of graphic organizers. My middle school students assumed the purpose of using a graphic organizer was to prove that they read the assignment. They saw it as a tangible piece of evidence to show they had done the invisible work of reading. Likewise, I developed intricate graphic organizers that were specific to the text we were reading rather than the reading skill we were trying to build. 

While those tools were lovely, I overcomplicated a simple ( and research-based ) concept: The purpose of graphic organizers is to help students interact with the text and organize their thoughts. Consequently, they also help students to purposely slow down. This counters the habit of skimming through “just to get it done.”

Graphic organizers and note-taking guides aren’t just busywork or a way for students to prove they read an assigned passage. The right graphic organizer can help students to identify sequence, separate cause from effect, or make inferences. Overall, no matter what the format of the graphic organizer , slowing down and writing something helps students make sense of what they read. That is the superpower of a graphic organizer. 

Similarly, when students write about what they read in science, social studies, and math, they learn the content better. While writing, students naturally slow down to retrieve and rehearse what they read. This helps them to remember it longer and make deeper connections. A simple (and low-prep) way to routinize writing is to employ conjunctions to help students connect content-based ideas.

Teachers in my district have had great success using sentence-level writing prompts from The Writing Revolution . One favorite approach can double as conversation starters and exit tickets using simple conjunctions. This exercise has the added benefit of giving students repeated practice writing complex sentences and providing a check for understanding for their teacher.

Leverage Comprehension Monitoring

Comprehension monitoring occurs when readers catch themselves losing track of what they read. Expert readers intuit this drift with minimal effort and tend to reread what they miss. Students who are still building their skills, however, need explicit instruction on how to purposely pause and make sure they’re getting it. In my classroom, writing or typing five-word summaries in the margins of each paragraph helped middle school students internalize a quick comprehension check and also develop an easy-to-transfer annotation skill. In pieces with frequent dialogue or short paragraphs, we’d write our five words about a hand’s breadth of text. 

The routine is so transferable that I found myself using it when reading Supreme Court cases for a recent doctoral class. The language was so complex, and sometimes droll, that writing five words or fewer in the margin of each paragraph helped me to monitor (and record) my own understanding. Stopping to write a few words will slow down the reading process, but it also helps to ensure that students read deeply enough so that the information sticks. This routine may be even more important when students read digitally, as new studies suggest that students’ reading comprehension may be impeded by reading onscreen .

The notion of reading more deeply gives renewed value to note-taking and think-alouds that have been used to support literacy for years. Young people’s reading habits are inevitably changing as new forms of entertainment and connection vie for their attention. Understanding when and how to take time for deeper reading is an antidote to the fast information that permeates students’ reading lives.

Readers, what have you learned from this article? Please share in the comment section, and, time permitting, also include your own strategies for building student comprehension skills.

research topic in reading comprehension

Reading print is better for comprehension, study finds

Leisure reading on paper helps with text comprehension better than reading on digital devices, according to a new study.

Driving the news: "The main conclusion is that leisure reading habits on screen are minimally related to reading comprehension," researchers at the University of Valencia found.

By the numbers: The researchers, who analyzed more than two dozen studies, said "the relationship between the frequency of reading printed texts and text comprehension is much higher (between 0.30 and 0.40) than what we found for leisure digital reading habits (0.05)."

  • "This means, for example, that if a student spends 10 hours reading books on paper, their comprehension will probably be 6 to 8 times greater than if they read on digital devices for the same amount of time," study co-authors Cristina Vargas and Ladislao Salmerón said .

Of note: The study also found that as students get older, the relationship between recreational reading on digital devices and text comprehension improves.

Details: The researchers analyzed 25 studies on reading comprehension published between 2000 and 2022, with more than 450,000 participants.

  • "One might have expected that reading for informational purposes (i.e., visiting Wikipedia or other educational websites; reading news, or reading e-books) would be much more positively related to comprehension, but this is not the case," one researcher said.
  • The study was published earlier this week in the Review of Educational Research .

Go deeper: Attempts to ban books at public libraries surge at record levels

Get the rundown of the biggest stories of the day with Axios Daily Essentials.

Reading print is better for comprehension, study finds

Reading Comprehension Research: Implications for Practice and Policy

Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach, measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading comprehension, international and national reading scores indicate stagnant growth for U.S. adolescents. In this article, we review the theoretical and empirical research in reading comprehension. We first explore different theoretical models for comprehension and then focus on components shown to be important across models that represent potential targets for instruction. In the last part of the article, we consider solutions for translating research to practice and policies for improving instruction. Improving reading scores will require a concerted and collaborative effort by researchers, educators, and policy makers with a focus on long-term solutions. An early and sustained focus on developing background knowledge, vocabulary, inference, and comprehension monitoring skills across development will be necessary to improve comprehension.

  • Related Documents

The South American Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Network (SAPASEN)

The present article describes the South American Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Network, which was designed to provide ongoing transnational empirical evidence about physical activity and sedentary behavior in South America. The first goal of this initiative was to form a representative body of researchers and policy makers from all South American countries (Argentina, Bolivia, Brazil, Chile, Colombia, Ecuador, Guyana, Paraguay, Peru, Suriname, Uruguay and Venezuela) to establish priorities and targets for the short, medium and long term. Examples are given of connecting physical activity and sedentary data from existing surveys in several of the partner countries. The main objective of the South American Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Network will be to impact policies on physical activity and sedentary behavior in South America according to the singularities of each country or region. By encouraging an inclusive and collaborative effort, we expect that the South American Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior Network will support the connection between researchers from South America as well as provide a better comprehension of the epidemiology of physical activity and sedentary behavior regionally.

IMPROVING THE STUDENTS’ READING COMPREHENSION ACHIEVEMENT IN DESCRIPTIVE TEXT THROUGH TEAM GAMES TOURNAMENT

This study attempted to improve the students’ reading comprehension achievement in descriptive text through Team Games Tournament (TGT) method. This study was conducted by using classroom action research. The subject of the research was class IX SMP PAB 10 Medan Estate which consisted of 25 students. The research was conducted in two cycles and the first cycle consisted of four meetings the second cycle consisted of two meetings. The instruments for collecting the data were quantitative data (reading evaluation) and qualitative data (diary note, observation sheet and interview sheet). Based on reading scores, students’ score kept improving in every evaluation. In the test I the mean was 65,33, in the test II the mean was 71,72 and the test III the mean was 84,54.Based on diary note, observation sheet and interview sheet, it was found that teaching-learning process ran well. Students were active, enthusiastic, and interested in reading. The result of the research showed that Team Games Tournament (TGT) method significantly improved students’ achievement in reading comprehension especially in reading descriptive text.

Some Simple Thoughts on Model Building for Developing Countries

The revival of interest in using models for developing countries for planning and policy advice makes it desirable to draw lessons from past experience. Simple models, focussed on specific issues, are to be preferred to large and comprehensive models. More empirical research is needed on supply and production functions for developing countries. Not statistical criteria but developmental considerations must determine the conclusions to be drawn from models. More explicit explanations of assumptions and modesty in presenting results of modelling work to policy makers are desirable.

Emotion Dysregulation in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is defined by deficits in social communication and interaction, and restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests. Although current diagnostic conceptualizations of ASD do not include emotional difficulties as core deficits, the disorder is associated with emotion dysregulation across the lifespan, with considerable implications for long-term psychological, social, and educational outcomes. The overarching goal of this chapter is to integrate existing knowledge of emotion dysregulation in ASD and identify areas for further investigation. The chapter reviews the prevalence and expressions of emotion dysregulation in ASD, discusses emerging theoretical models that frame emotion dysregulation as an inherent (rather than associated) feature of ASD, presents neurobiological findings and mechanisms related to emotion dysregulation in ASD, and identifies continuing controversies and resulting research priorities.

We’re Not That Choosy: Emerging Evidence of a Progression Bias in Romantic Relationships

Dating is widely thought of as a test phase for romantic relationships, during which new romantic partners carefully evaluate each other for long-term fit. However, this cultural narrative assumes that people are well equipped to reject poorly suited partners. In this article, we argue that humans are biased toward pro-relationship decisions—decisions that favor the initiation, advancement, and maintenance of romantic relationships. We first review evidence for a progression bias in the context of relationship initiation, investment, and breakup decisions. We next consider possible theoretical underpinnings—both evolutionary and cultural—that may explain why getting into a relationship is often easier than getting out of one, and why being in a less desirable relationship is often preferred over being in no relationship at all. We discuss potential boundary conditions that the phenomenon may have, as well as its implications for existing theoretical models of mate selection and relationship development.

Solo self-employment, entrepreneurial subjectivity and the security–precarity continuum: Evidence from private tutors in the supplementary education industry

Entrepreneurship is regarded by policy makers and politicians as an accelerant for economic development. Economic geography demonstrates that rather than stimulating entrepreneurship in general, policy makers should support specific forms of entrepreneurship that fuel wider growth. The paper's original contribution is to insist that entrepreneurship research must also explore less growth-oriented, but crucially very widespread, forms of entrepreneurial activity. The paper therefore places solo self-employment – the self-employed without employees – centre stage as an exemplar of this trend. Research is presented on private tutors who run businesses from home, offering children one-to-one tuition in the burgeoning supplementary education industry. The paper scrutinises the causes, configuration and consequences of such solo self-employment as an economically marginal, but numerically dominant, form of entrepreneurship. In so doing, it makes three conceptual advances in the exploration of heterogeneous entrepreneurship. First, in examining why individuals become self-employed, the paper moves beyond classic efforts to understand entrepreneurship through binary push/pull mechanisms in models of occupational choice. Instead, the analysis demonstrates the importance of risk in entrepreneurship and paid employment, highlighting the multiple pathways into solo self-employment as opportunities and constraints coalesce in individual's lives. Secondly, in considering how the solo self-employed think about business, the research breaks through conventional definitions of entrepreneurship to demonstrate that solo self-employment involves a distinctively entrepreneurial subjectivity and practices. Thirdly, by investigating the consequences of solo self-employment, the findings transcend dualist interpretations of self-employment as the realm of entrepreneurial wealth or economic precarity, highlighting instead a security–precarity continuum in immediate and long-term outcomes.

OP48 Nursing Requirements In Long-Term Care: A Health Technology Assessment

IntroductionThe objectives of this study were to systematically review published research on the relationship between nursing staff coverage, care hours, and quality of care (QoC) in long-term care (LTC) facilities; and to conduct a real world evidence (RWE) analysis using Alberta real world data (RWD) to inform policy makers on whether any amendments could be made to current regulations.MethodsA systematic review (SR) of research evidence published between January 2000 and May 2018 on the relationship between nursing staff coverage, care hours, and QoC in LTC facilities was conducted. Panel data regressions using available RWD from Alberta, Canada, were performed to assess associations between nursing care hours and LTC outcomes. Outcomes of interest included quality indicators related to resident outcomes, hospital admissions, emergency room visits and family satisfaction. Nursing care hours considered in SR and RWE analysis included those provided by registered nurses (RNs) and licensed practical nurses (LPNs).ResultsThe SR found inconsistent and poor quality evidence relevant to the questions of interest, indicating a great uncertainty about the association between nursing staff time and type of coverage and QoC. Although some positive indications were suggested, major weaknesses of reviewed studies limited interpretation of SR results. RWE analysis found that impact of care hours on LTC outcomes was heterogeneous, dependent on outcome measurements. There was evidence that total staff, RN, and LPN hours had positive effects on some resident outcomes and magnitude of effect differed for different nursing staff.ConclusionsNo definitive conclusion could be drawn on whether changing nursing staff time or nursing staff coverage models would affect residents’ outcomes based on the research evidence gathered in the SR. RWE analysis helped to fill a gap in the available published literature and allowed policy makers to better understand the impact of revising current regulations based on actual outcomes.

EU Labor Market Inequalities and Sustainable Development Goals

Reducing inequalities for EU citizens and promoting upward convergence is one of the priorities on the agenda of the European Commission and, certainly, inequality will be a very important public policy issue for years to come. Through this research we aim to investigate EU labor market inequalities, reflected by the specific indicators proposed for Goal 8 assumed by the 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development, based on cluster analysis for all the 27 Member States. The research results showed encouraging results from the perspective of convergence in the EU labor market, but also revealed a number of analyzed variable effects that manifested regional inequalities that were generated in the medium and long term. Based on the observations made, we want to provide information for policy-makers, business practitioners, and academics so as to constitute solid ground for identifying good practices and proposing to implement policies aimed at reducing existing inequalities and supporting sustainable development.

Small-Scale Deliberation and Mass Democracy: A Systematic Review of the Spillover Effects of Deliberative Minipublics

Deliberative minipublics are popular tools to address the current crisis in democracy. However, it remains ambiguous to what degree these small-scale forums matter for mass democracy. In this study, we ask the question to what extent minipublics have “spillover effects” on lay citizens—that is, long-term effects on participating citizens and effects on non-participating citizens. We answer this question by means of a systematic review of the empirical research on minipublics’ spillover effects published before 2019. We identify 60 eligible studies published between 1999 and 2018 and provide a synthesis of the empirical results. We show that the evidence for most spillover effects remains tentative because the relevant body of empirical evidence is still small. Based on the review, we discuss the implications for democratic theory and outline several trajectories for future research.

Shared socioeconomic pathways for climate change research in Finland: co-developing extended SSP narratives for agriculture

AbstractShared socioeconomic pathways (SSPs), developed at global scale, comprise narrative descriptions and quantifications of future world developments that are intended for climate change scenario analysis. However, their extension to national and regional scales can be challenging. Here, we present SSP narratives co-developed with stakeholders for the agriculture and food sector in Finland. These are derived from intensive discussions at a workshop attended by approximately 39 participants offering a range of sectoral perspectives. Using general background descriptions of the SSPs for Europe, facilitated discussions were held in parallel for each of four SSPs reflecting very different contexts for the development of the sector up to 2050 and beyond. Discussions focused on five themes from the perspectives of consumers, producers and policy-makers, included a joint final session and allowed for post-workshop feedback. Results reflect careful sector-based, national-level interpretations of the global SSPs from which we have constructed consensus narratives. Our results also show important critical remarks and minority viewpoints. Interesting features of the Finnish narratives compared to the global SSP narratives include greater emphasis on environmental quality; significant land abandonment in SSPs with reduced livestock production and increased plant-based diets; continued need for some farm subsidies across all SSPs and opportunities for diversifying domestic production under scenarios of restricted trade. Our results can contribute to the development of more detailed national long-term scenarios for food and agriculture that are both relevant for local stakeholders and researchers as well as being consistent with global scenarios being applied internationally.

Export Citation Format

Share document.

research topic in reading comprehension

2024 cohort of New Generation Thinkers announced by BBC and AHRC

Head on chalkboard with light bulb notes inside.

16 May 2024

Ten of the UK’s most promising arts and humanities early career researchers have been announced as this year’s BBC and AHRC New Generation Thinkers.

Every year, a nationwide search is held for the best new arts and humanities academics with ideas that will resonate with a wider audience on BBC radio. From hundreds of applications, these 10 New Generation Thinkers represent some of the best early career researchers in the country.

They will be given the opportunity to share their pioneering research with BBC Radio 4 listeners. They will also be provided with unique access to training and support from the Arts and Humanities Research Council (AHRC) and the BBC.

The 2024 New Generation Thinkers will bring new insights into diverse topics, with research including the:

  • possible existence of the multiverse
  • future of black literature
  • surprisingly dark history of Technicolor film
  • search for the greatest philosopher who never existed

The names of the 10 researchers were announced as part of a New Thinking episode of the Arts & Ideas podcast hosted by former New Generation Thinker Eleanor Rosamund Barraclough.

The 2024 New Generation Thinkers.

Shaping thought and discussion

Professor Christopher Smith, AHRC Executive Chair says:

The New Generation Thinkers programme brings interesting, important ideas into the public consciousness, shaping thought and discussion. From fundamental questions about the nature of reality to how political propagandists harnessed the seductive power of Technicolor, and the impact of imprisonment on mothers and children, and the most challenging problems of our day, these are ideas of thrilling originality and importance. These ten brilliant, original thinkers demonstrate the ability of the arts and humanities to help us to better understand both ourselves and the world around us.

A new home of listeners

Matthew Dodd, Commissioning Editor, Arts, BBC Radio 3 and 4 said:

We’re looking forward to working with ten of the most promising early-career academics. Each year the New Generation Thinker scheme brings radio production teams a wave of stimulating and thought-provoking contributors who have a passion for public engagement. After over a decade of successful partnership with BBC Radio 3, it’s great to be bringing their ideas to a new home of listeners on BBC Radio 4, the biggest speech radio station in the UK, where they’ll find a wide audience.

Further information

Childcare: a history of love and work.

Dr Emily Baughan, The University of Sheffield

Dr Emily Baughan is interested in how children are cared for, and by whom. Her first book ‘Saving the Children’, explored the origins of a global humanitarian movement focused on the very young. She is currently working on a new project, titled ‘Childcare: A History of Love and Work’.

Emily’s research into two centuries of childhood includes baby farms in Victorian London and children’s prison camps in colonial Kenya. She has examined the origins of international adoption, and radical feminist nurseries in 1970s Italy and the US. She is currently writing about maternity care and childcare in present-day Britain.

Emily is a senior lecturer in history at The University of Sheffield.

Serving the self: performance at work

Dr Jaswinder Blackwell-Pal, Queen Mary, University of London

For many of us, the demand to perform is now part of our working lives, from serving the customer in hospitality, to prioritising ‘soft skills’ in the office, to the way care is performed in education and health.

Dr Jaswinder Blackwell-Pal’s work explores how these imperatives to present certain feelings, characteristics or behaviours have become embedded within contemporary work.

Combining her expertise in performance and actor training with accounts drawn from managers, customers and employees, her research asks why so much of modern work feels like putting on an act.

Jaswinder is a lecturer in the Drama Department at Queen Mary, University of London. Her research has been published in Platform, The Journal of Dramatic Theory and Criticism, and in Bloomsbury’s History of Emotions series.

Reading, sentimentality, and empathy

Dr Janine Bradbury, University of York

Dr Janine Bradbury is an award-winning poet and critic. She is a Senior Lecturer in Contemporary Writing and Culture at the University of York. Janine completed a part-time PhD in African American women’s writing at The University of Sheffield (2008 to 2015).

Janine’s research and creative practice is wide-ranging. Her work on Grace Jones, American professional wrestling, passing-for-white, comedy, Toni Morrison, and motherhood has been published by Bloomsbury, Palgrave Macmillan, Routledge, the Guardian, the Young Vic Theatre, The Emma Press, Magma, and Oxford Poetry (and elsewhere). She has contributed to programmes on BBC Radio 4 (Woman’s Hour and Great Lives).

Her current work explores the relationship between love, feeling, and reading (words, people, situations). Her debut poetry pamphlet ‘Sometimes Real Love Comes Quick and Easy’ (2024, ignitionpress) explores sentimentality.

Scriptocurrency: the new language of British nature poets of colour

Jade Cuttle, University of Cambridge

Jade Cuttle is an award-winning writer and AHRC-funded researcher at Cambridge, studying the coinings of British nature poets of colour. Applying her experience in metal detecting and mudlarking to the field of literature, she unearths evidence of shared currency and an overlooked tradition.

She is writing a book called Silthood, a term she coined to explore ancient connections to the earth. Supervised by Robert Macfarlane, her research precedes the publication of Nature Matters: New Poetries by Black and Asian Writers of the diaspora.

Jade won a ‘30 To Watch: Journalism Award’ for her Arts Commissioning Editor work at The Times, with televised appearances on BBC One, ITV and Look North. She’s also a historical reenactor, one of Britain’s first female warriors of colour.

Sound, listening and intimacy in everyday life

Dr Jacob Downs, University of Oxford

Everywhere you look, people are using intimate sound technologies to reshape and enrich their experiences of the world. They surround themselves in the ‘bubbles’ of noise-cancelling headphones to listen to podcasts, audiobooks, and whispered pop vocals. Why are we seeking out intimate sound-worlds so much in the 21st century?

In his research, Dr Jacob Downs places contemporary listening under the microscope to investigate the effects of new sound media on our everyday lives.

Dr Jacob Downs is departmental lecturer in music at the University of Oxford, where he researches and lectures on topics in 20th and 21st-century music and sound.

He is currently writing two books (one on headphone listening, the other on environmentalist music) and articles on subjects ranging from artificial intelligence-generated music to Beyoncé’s latest album.

In search of Zera Yacob: the greatest philosopher who never existed

Jonathan Egid, King’s College London

Philosophers often question the nature of being, but what about when the existence of the philosopher themself is in question? Jonathan Egid has spent the past few years digging through the archives on the trail of a brilliant and neglected thinker from 17th century Ethiopia, and the question of whether he existed.

Exploring the philosophy and fraught politics of the Hatata Zera Yacob, a philosophical autobiography that may or may not be a forgery, his research examines the contemporary resonances of this enigmatic text for our age of violent conflict. It explores the role it can play in debates about diversifying and decolonising philosophy.

Jonathan Egid is a PhD candidate at King’s College London and a research fellow at the Freie Universität in Berlin. His essays on philosophy, history and literature appear regularly in the Times Literary Supplement, Aeon and elsewhere.

He is currently working on two books, one on the Hatata Zera Yacob and another on Jewish life in postwar Poland. He also hosts the podcast and interview series ‘Philosophising In…’ on philosophy in lesser-studied languages.

Peeling off motherhood

Dr Shona Minson, University of Oxford

Originally from Belfast, Dr Shona Minson is an award-winning criminologist at the University of Oxford, whose first career as a family and criminal barrister led her to explore families and punishment.

Shona is a unique voice and a leading authority on how to do justice better for women and their children. She contributes regularly to public conversations about the wider consequences of punishment, and women’s justice issues.

Her innovative short films based on interviews with children, mothers and grandmothers affected by maternal imprisonment, changed legal professional practice in the UK and overseas.

Her first book highlighted the lack of concern given to children whose mothers are imprisoned and the next examines the impact on society when the motherhood of criminalised women is disregarded.

The dark side of the rainbow

Dr Kirsty Sinclair Dootson, University College London

Dr Kirsty Sinclair Dootson is interested in the politics of making images in colour. Exploring the raw ingredients, labour and technologies that go into making colourful images, her research asks: can colour be immoral? Dangerous? Bad for the environment? She asks: is there a dark side of the rainbow?

From Technicolor musicals to Instagram, her work helps us understand how colours that appear to be frivolous distractions might also harbour deep political power.

Dr Kirsty Sinclair Dootson is a lecturer in film and media at UCL. She received her PhD in Film Studies and History of Art from Yale University.

Her award-winning book ‘The Rainbow’s Gravity’ reveals how modern colour media technologies transformed the way Britain saw itself and its empire. She currently co-convenes the AHRC research network ‘Bombay Film Colour’.

Exploring the multiverse

Dr Jack Symes, Durham University

Jack Symes is a public philosopher and researcher at Durham University. His public work explores all areas of philosophy, while his academic research lies at the intersection of metaphysics and religion.

Jack’s current project investigates the multiverse: whether our universe exists as one among many and, if so, the significance of our place within it.

Jack is the host of The Panpsycast Philosophy Podcast, one of the UK’s most popular philosophy programmes. He also edits Bloomsbury’s Talking about Philosophy book series, which features contributions from the field’s leading thinkers.

Jack’s books include:

  • Philosophers on Consciousness: Talking about the Mind (2022)
  • Philosophers on God: Talking about Existence (2024)
  • Defeating the Evil-God Challenge: In Defence of God’s Goodness (2024)

Moving images: picturing crises differently

Dr Becca Voelcker, Goldsmiths, University of London

In a contemporary media landscape saturated by divisive representations of social and ecological crises, Dr Becca Voelcker’s research explores artistic and filmic responses and tries to stimulate visual literacy.

Becca lectures in art and film history at Goldsmiths, University of London, specialising in representations of land since the 1970s. Becca received her PhD from Harvard University in 2021.

She is currently writing her first book, a cross-cultural history of eco-political cinema based in 10 locations including Wales, where Becca grew up, and Japan, where she lived as a young adult.

Alongside research, Becca writes for ‘Sight and Sound’ and ‘Frieze’, introduces films at the British Film Institute, and serves on film festival juries.

Top image:  Credit: yangwenshuang, E+ via Getty Images

Share this page

  • Share this page on Twitter
  • Share this page on LinkedIn
  • Share this page on Facebook

This is the website for UKRI: our seven research councils, Research England and Innovate UK. Let us know if you have feedback or would like to help improve our online products and services .

U.S. flag

A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

A lock ( ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Risk Factors
  • Health Equity
  • Cancer Data and Statistics
  • Cancer Research
  • Resource Library
  • Types of Cancer
  • HPV and Cancer
  • Cancer Research Citation Search Tips
  • Initiatives

What to know

The following tips will help you use the Cancer Research Citation Search tool.

The Cancer Research Citation Search tool allows you to search for scientific articles authored or coauthored by researchers from CDC's Division of Cancer Prevention and Control (DCPC) starting in 2000.

The articles themselves are not stored in the search tool. Citation titles are linked to the National Institutes of Health's PubMed database or PubMed Central archive, or CDC's journals Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR) and Preventing Chronic Disease. If available, a digital object identifier (DOI) link is also provided.

Selecting the search criteria

If you leave all fields empty and click Search, you will retrieve the entire list of articles.

Use the Keywords field to search for specific words in the title of the article.

Match any word

Select Match any word to retrieve articles that contain any of the words in the Keywords field in the article title. For example, if you type "prostate cancer" in the Keywords field using this option, you will retrieve all articles containing either "prostate" or "cancer" in the title (which would be most of the articles).

Exact match

Select Exact match to retrieve articles that contain the exact phrase in the Keywords field in the article title. For example, if you type "prostate cancer" in the Keywords field using this option, you will retrieve all articles containing the phrase "prostate cancer" in the title, but not articles containing "cancer of the prostate" in the title.

Match all words

Select Match all words to retrieve articles that contain all of the words in the Keywords field in the article title. For example, if you type "prostate cancer" in the Keyword field using this option, you will retrieve all articles containing both "prostate" and "cancer" in the title.

Author last name

Use the Author last name field to search for articles by a particular author. Note that since many articles are collaborations between researchers from DCPC and other institutions, the author may not be a DCPC researcher.

DCPC first authors

Check the DCPC first authors checkbox to retrieve only articles that were first-authored by a DCPC researcher.

Each article is assigned to one or more topics manually when the citation is entered. To retrieve articles that have been assigned to one or more topics, click the checkboxes corresponding to the topics you want.

If you select multiple topics, you will retrieve a list of articles that have been assigned to all of the topics you selected. If an article doesn't fit in any topic, it is assigned to the Other Research topic.

Journal name

Use the Journal name field to search for articles that were published in a particular journal.

Publication/calendar year between

Use these two fields to select a beginning and ending calendar year of publication. For example:

  • If you want to retrieve articles that were published between 2021 and 2024, select 2021 from the first dropdown list and 2024 from the second dropdown list.
  • If you want to retrieve articles published during one year, select that year from both dropdown lists. For example, if you select 2024 from both dropdown lists, you will retrieve articles published in 2024.

Fiscal year between

Use these two fields to select a beginning and ending fiscal year of publication. CDC's fiscal years begin October 1 and end September 30. For example, fiscal year 2024 began October 1, 2023 and ends September 30, 2024. See the instructions for Publication/calendar year between for more information.

Note that you can include either calendar years or fiscal years in your search criteria, but not both.

When you have entered all of your search criteria, click the Search button. The list of results appears.

Viewing the search results

Sorting the search results.

When you have retrieved your results, the default sort order is ascending alphabetical (A to Z, 1 to 9) by article title. You can sort the results in different ways by clicking on the column titles. To sort the column in descending order (Z to A, 9 to 1), click the down arrow in the column title.

Please note that if you choose to sort the list by the Author column, the list is sorted alphabetically by the first author.

Setting the number of search results to view per page

By default, 25 results are shown on each page of the list. To increase the number of results to show on each page, select the desired number from the Show citations dropdown list at the top or bottom of the page.

Accessing and reading an article

To read an article in the search results list:

  • Click on the article title. A new browser window will open and show either the abstract of the article (if available) in PubMed, or the full text of the article in PubMed Central, MMWR, or Preventing Chronic Disease. The PubMed abstract will include links (if available) to the full text of the article; however, many journals charge a fee to access the full text.
  • Click on the DOI (if available). A new browser window will open and show the article in a journal.

Learn how to lower your cancer risk and what CDC is doing to prevent and control cancer.

COMMENTS

  1. Reading Comprehension Research: Implications for Practice and Policy

    Similarly, the RAND reading model, another influential reading framework for research and practice, defined reading comprehension as the process of "extracting and constructing meaning through interaction and involvement with written language" (RAND Reading Study Group, 2002, p. 11). Specifically, reading comprehension is the interaction ...

  2. The Science of Reading Comprehension Instruction

    Decades of research offer important understandings about the nature of comprehension and its development. Drawing on both classic and contemporary research, in this article, we identify some key understandings about reading comprehension processes and instruction, including these: Comprehension instruction should begin early, teaching word-reading and bridging skills (including ...

  3. The Use of New Technologies for Improving Reading Comprehension

    Introduction. Reading comprehension is a fundamental cognitive ability for children, that supports school achievement and successively participation in most areas of adult life (Hulme and Snowling, 2011).Therefore, children with learning disabilities (LD) and special educational needs who show difficulties in text comprehension, sometimes also in association with other problems, may have an ...

  4. A Meta-Analysis of Interventions to Improve Reading Comprehension

    Her research interests include teacher education, special education policy, and teacher shortages, with a focus in reading and using qualitative research designs. Prior to her work at Rhode Island College, Kelly was a special educator for over 13 years in a large, multicultural high school located in the Northeastern United States.

  5. Reading Comprehension Research: Implications for Practice and Policy

    Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach, measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading ...

  6. Handbook of Research on Reading Comprehension

    The Handbook of Research on Reading Comprehension assembles researchers of reading comprehension, literacy, educational psychology, psychology, and neuroscience to document the most recent research on the topic. It summarizes the current body of research on theory, methods, instruction, and assessment, including coverage of landmark studies. Designed to deepen understanding of how past ...

  7. How the Science of Reading Informs 21st‐Century Education

    The science of reading should be informed by an evolving evidence base built upon the scientific method. Decades of basic research and randomized controlled trials of interventions and instructional routines have formed a substantial evidence base to guide best practices in reading instruction, reading intervention, and the early identification of at-risk readers.

  8. Levels of Reading Comprehension in Higher Education: Systematic Review

    This review is a guide to direct future research, broadening the study focus on the level of reading comprehension using digital technology, experimental designs, second languages, and investigations that relate reading comprehension with other factors (gender, cognitive abilities, etc.) that can explain the heterogeneity in the different ...

  9. What Research Tells Us About Reading, Comprehension, and Comprehension

    For many years, reading instruction was based on a concept of reading as the application of a set of isolated skills such as identifying words, finding main ideas, identifying cause and effect relationships, comparing and contrasting and sequencing. Comprehension was viewed as the mastery of these skills. One important classroom study conducted ...

  10. The Role of Background Knowledge in Reading Comprehension: A Critical

    The Role of Domain Knowledge. The Construction-Integration model identifies a critical role for background knowledge in reading (Kintsch, Citation 1998; Kintsch & Van Dijk, Citation 1978).Knowledge can be classified according to its specificity; background knowledge comprises all of the world knowledge that the reader brings to the task of reading. This can include episodic (events ...

  11. Journal of Research in Reading

    It is a peer-reviewed journal principally devoted to reports of original empirical research in reading and closely related fields (e.g., spoken language, writing), and to informed reviews of relevant literature. The Journal welcomes papers on the learning, teaching, and use of literacy in adults or children in a variety of contexts, with a ...

  12. PDF Reading Comprehension, What We Know: A Review of Research ...

    This review of research concerning reading comprehension provides incites into what has been learned from 1995 to the present. Reading ... Reading is an activity performed to develop an understanding of a subject or topic. Reading is an essential skill that individuals need to process in order to be successful in life. Reading keeps individuals ...

  13. (PDF) Improving Students' Reading Comprehension Skills Using Peer

    The research objective was to improve reading comprehension skills using the peer- assisted learning strategy. The subject of the study was the forty-six (46)

  14. reading comprehension research: Topics by Science.gov

    Reading comprehension of school-aged students is an important topic of research; however, research on the reading comprehension of adult foreign/second language learners whose first language is English is limited, especially studies investigating the reading comprehension strategies that readers of different proficiency levels use when they…

  15. New Research on Reading Comprehension (and 5 Tips for Teachers

    The researchers discovered a "knowledge threshold" when it comes to reading comprehension: If students were unfamiliar with 59 percent of the terms in a topic, their ability to understand the text was "compromised.". In the study, 3,534 high school students were presented with a list of 44 terms and asked to identify whether each was ...

  16. The Effectiveness of Reading Strategies on Reading Comprehension

    Abstract —This research aimed to investigate the effectiveness. of reading strategies on reading comprehension of the second. year English major students who enrolled to study English. Reading ...

  17. Synthesis of Research / Reading Comprehension: What Works

    Research of the late 1970s and early '80s consistently revealed a strong reciprocal relationship between prior knowledge and reading comprehension ability. The more one already knows, the more one comprehends; and the more one comprehends, the more one learns new knowledge to enable comprehension of an even greater and broader array of topics ...

  18. PDF Improving Reading Comprehension

    The teacher researchers intended to improve reading comprehension by using higher-order thinking skills such as predicting, making connections, visualizing, inferring, questioning, and summarizing. In their classrooms the teacher researchers modeled these strategies through the think-aloud process and graphic organizers.

  19. Reading comprehension research: Implications for practice and policy

    Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach, measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading comprehension, international and national reading scores indicate stagnant growth for U.S. adolescents. In this article, we review the theoretical and empirical research in reading comprehension. We first ...

  20. Boosting Students' Reading Comprehension by Slowing Down

    Similarly, when students write about what they read in science, social studies, and math, they learn the content better. While writing, students naturally slow down to retrieve and rehearse what they read. This helps them to remember it longer and make deeper connections. A simple (and low-prep) way to routinize writing is to employ ...

  21. Reading print is better for comprehension, study finds

    Leisure reading on paper helps with text comprehension better than reading on digital devices, according to a new study. Driving the news: "The main conclusion is that leisure reading habits on ...

  22. Vocabulary and Reading Comprehension Revisited: Evidence for High-, Mid

    Although a substantial number of studies have found vocabulary knowledge to be a significant predictor of reading success in L2 learners and have established certain vocabulary size and lexical coverage targets for comprehension (e.g., Hazenberg & Hulstijn, 1996; Hu & Nation, 2000; Laufer, 1992a; Nation, 2006; Schmitt, Jiang, & Grabe, 2011), most of those studies have predominantly focused on ...

  23. Reading Comprehension Research: Implications for Practice and Policy

    Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach, measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading comprehension, international and national reading scores indicate stagnant growth for U.S. adolescents. In this article, we review the theoretical and empirical ...

  24. 2024 cohort of New Generation Thinkers announced by BBC and AHRC

    The 2024 New Generation Thinkers will bring new insights into diverse topics, with research including the: possible existence of the multiverse; ... Reading, sentimentality, and empathy. Dr Janine Bradbury, University of York. Dr Janine Bradbury is an award-winning poet and critic. She is a Senior Lecturer in Contemporary Writing and Culture at ...

  25. Reading Comprehension Research: Implications for Practice and Policy

    Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach, measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading comprehension, international and national reading scores indicate stagnant growth for U.S. adolescents.

  26. Cancer Research Citation Search Tips

    Overview. The Cancer Research Citation Search tool allows you to search for scientific articles authored or coauthored by researchers from CDC's Division of Cancer Prevention and Control (DCPC) starting in 2000.. The articles themselves are not stored in the search tool. Citation titles are linked to the National Institutes of Health's PubMed database or PubMed Central archive, or CDC's ...